Content deleted Content added
Neutronstar2 (talk | contribs) mNo edit summary |
GreenC bot (talk | contribs) Rescued 3 archive links; reformat 1 link. Wayback Medic 2.5 |
||
Line 162:
==Probabilistic interpretation==
An [[associative memory (psychology)|associative memory]] system using sparse, distributed representations can be reinterpreted as an [[Importance sampling|importance sampler]], a [[Monte Carlo method|Monte
Carlo]] method of approximating [[Bayesian inference]].<ref>Abbott, Joshua T., Jessica B. Hamrick, and Thomas L. Griffiths. "[https://web.archive.org/web/20170911115555/https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/7f50/8bb0bf0010884a4be72f2774635514fc58ec.pdf Approximating Bayesian inference with a sparse distributed memory system]." Proceedings of the 35th annual conference of the cognitive science society. 2013.</ref> The SDM can be considered a Monte Carlo approximation to a multidimensional [[conditional probability]] integral. The SDM will produce acceptable responses from a training set when this approximation is valid, that is, when the training set contains sufficient data to provide good estimates of the underlying [[Joint probability distribution|joint probabilities]] and there are enough Monte Carlo samples to obtain an accurate estimate of the integral.<ref>{{cite book|doi=10.1109/ijcnn.1989.118597|chapter=A conditional probability interpretation of Kanerva's sparse distributed memory|title=International Joint Conference on Neural Networks|pages=415–417|volume=1|year=1989|last1=Anderson|s2cid=13935339}}</ref>
==Biological plausibility==
[[Sparse coding]] may be a general strategy of neural systems to augment memory capacity. To adapt to their environments, animals must learn which stimuli are associated with rewards or punishments and distinguish these reinforced stimuli from similar but irrelevant ones. Such task requires implementing stimulus-specific [[associative memory (psychology)|associative memories]] in which only a few neurons out of a [[Neural ensemble|population]] respond to any given stimulus and each neuron responds to only a few stimuli out of all possible stimuli.
Theoretical work on SDM by Kanerva has suggested that sparse coding increases the capacity of associative memory by reducing overlap between representations. Experimentally, sparse representations of sensory information have been observed in many systems, including vision,<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Vinje | first1 = WE | last2 = Gallant | first2 = JL | year = 2000 | title = Sparse coding and decorrelation in primary visual cortex during natural vision | url = https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/3efc/4ac8f70edde57661b908105f4fd21a43fbab.pdf | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20170911115737/https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/3efc/4ac8f70edde57661b908105f4fd21a43fbab.pdf | url-status = dead | archive-date = 2017-09-11 | journal = Science | volume = 287 | issue = 5456| pages = 1273–1276 | pmid = 10678835 | doi = 10.1126/science.287.5456.1273 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.456.2467 | bibcode = 2000Sci...287.1273V | s2cid = 13307465 }}</ref> audition,<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Hromádka | first1 = T | last2 = Deweese | first2 = MR | last3 = Zador | first3 = AM | year = 2008 | title = Sparse representation of sounds in the unanesthetized auditory cortex | journal = PLOS Biol | volume = 6 | issue = 1| page = e16 | pmid = 18232737 | doi=10.1371/journal.pbio.0060016 | pmc=2214813}}</ref> touch,<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Crochet | first1 = S | last2 = Poulet | first2 = JFA | last3 = Kremer | first3 = Y | last4 = Petersen | first4 = CCH | year = 2011 | title = Synaptic mechanisms underlying sparse coding of active touch | journal = Neuron | volume = 69 | issue = 6| pages = 1160–1175 | pmid = 21435560 | doi=10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.022| s2cid = 18528092 }}</ref> and olfaction.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Ito | first1 = I | last2 = Ong | first2 = RCY | last3 = Raman | first3 = B | last4 = Stopfer | first4 = M | year = 2008 | title = Sparse odor representation and olfactory learning | journal = Nat Neurosci | volume = 11 | issue = 10| pages = 1177–1184 | pmid = 18794840 | pmc=3124899 | doi=10.1038/nn.2192}}</ref> However, despite the accumulating evidence for widespread sparse coding and theoretical arguments for its importance, a demonstration that sparse coding improves the stimulus-specificity of associative memory has been lacking until recently.
Some progress has been made in 2014 by [[Gero Miesenböck]]'s lab at the [[University of Oxford]] analyzing [[Drosophila]] [[Olfactory system]].<ref>A sparse memory is a precise memory. Oxford Science blog. 28 Feb 2014. http://www.ox.ac.uk/news/science-blog/sparse-memory-precise-memory</ref>
Line 243:
===Object indexing in computer vision===
[[Dana H. Ballard]]'s lab<ref>Rao, Rajesh PN, and Dana H. Ballard. "[https://web.archive.org/web/20170911115922/https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/b918/b2326656a3661689e6bf3b6de9a8245d87ac.pdf Object indexing using an iconic sparse distributed memory]." Computer Vision, 1995. Proceedings., Fifth International Conference on. IEEE, 1995.</ref> demonstrated a general-purpose object indexing technique for [[computer vision]] that combines the virtues of [[principal component analysis]] with the favorable matching properties of high-dimensional spaces to achieve high precision recognition. The indexing algorithm uses an [[active vision]] system in conjunction with a modified form of SDM and provides a platform for learning the association between an object's appearance and its identity.
==Extensions==
|