Content deleted Content added
Added Scruton comment. |
SimLibrarian (talk | contribs) mNo edit summary Tags: Mobile edit Mobile app edit iOS app edit |
||
(5 intermediate revisions by 4 users not shown) | |||
Line 1:
{{Short description|Essay by Fred Lerdahl}}
'''"Cognitive Constraints on Compositional Systems"''' is an essay by [[Fred Lerdahl]] that cites [[Pierre Boulez]]'s ''[[Le Marteau sans
==Constraints
Lerdahl's constraints on artificial compositional grammars are:
===Event sequences===
*''Constraint 1'': The musical surface must be capable of being parsed into a sequence of discrete events.
**[counterexample: [[György Ligeti|Ligeti]], [[computer music]]]
*''Constraint 2'': The musical surface must be available for [[hierarchical]] structuring by the listening grammar.
**[through grouping structure, metrical structure, time-span reduction, and prolongational reduction. "Associational" factors such as [[Motif (music)|motivic]] development and [[timbre|timbral]] relations are ignored, but for "Timbral Hierarchies" see Lerdahl 1987]{{incomplete short citation|date=December 2021|reason=Or is 1988 meant?}}
*''Constraint 3'': The establishment of local grouping boundaries requires the presence of salient distinctive transitions at the musical surface.
**[counterexample: [[minimal music]]]
Line 16 ⟶ 19:
*''Constraint 8'': The projection of a prolongational tree depends on a corresponding time-span tree in conjunction with a set of stability conditions.
==
*''Constraint 9'': Stability conditions must operate on a fixed collection of elements.
Line 22 ⟶ 25:
*''Constraint 10'': [[Interval (music)|Intervals]] between elements of a collection arranged along a scale should fall within a certain range of magnitude.
*''Constraint 11'': A [[pitch (music)|pitch]] collection should recur at the [[octave]] to produce [[pitch class]]es.
**
*''Constraint 12'': There must be a strong [[psychoacoustic]] basis for stability conditions. For pitch collections, that requires intervals that proceed gradually from very small to comparatively large frequency [[ratio]]s.
**
*''Constraint 13'': Division of the octave into equal parts facilitates [[transposition (music)|transposition]] and reduces memory load.
**
*''Constraint 14'': Assume pitch sets of ''n''-fold equal divisions of the octave. Then subsets that satisfy uniqueness, coherence, and simplicity will facilitate ___location within the overall pitch space.
**[only certain divisions of the octave, 12 and 20 included, allow uniqueness, coherence, and transpositional simplicity, and only the diatonic and pentatonic subsets of the 12-tone chromatic set follow these constraints (Balzano, 1980, 1982){{incomplete short citation|date=December 2021}}]
===Pitch space===
*''Constraint 15'': Any but the most primitive stability conditions must be susceptible to multidimensional representation, where spatial distance correlates with cognitive distance.
*''Constraint 16'': Levels of pitch space must be sufficiently available from musical surfaces to be internalized.
*''Constraint 17'': A reductionally organized pitch space is needed to express the [[steps and skips]] by which cognitive distance is measured and to express degrees of melodic completeness.
**[completedness resembles implication-realization theory (Meyer, 1973{{incomplete short citation|date=December 2021}} and Narmour, 1977{{incomplete short citation|date=December 2021}}), the ''Zug'', ''[[Urlinie]]'', and ''[[Bassbrechung]]'' ([[Heinrich Schenker|Schenker]]){{incomplete short citation|date=December 2021}}.]
He concludes, "Some of these constraints seem to me binding, others optional. Constraints 9–12 are essential for the very existence of stability conditions. Constraints 13–17, on the other hand, can be variously jettisoned." Examples given are [[Carnatic music|South Indian music]], which doesn't [[modulation (music)|modulate]] and isn't equally tempered (13 & 14), and music such as that of [[Claude Debussy]], [[Béla Bartók]], and others who "have developed consonance-dissonance patterns directly from the [[total chromatic]]" (14–17).
Line 44 ⟶ 47:
*''Aesthetic Claim 2'': The best music arises from an alliance of a compositional grammar with the listening grammar.
To these ends he proposes the use of the terms "[[complexity]]" and "complicatedness", complexity being hierarchical structural richness, and complicatedness being "numerous non-redundant events per unit time." On Lerdahl's view complexity has aesthetic value, while complicatedness is neutral. He writes, "All sorts of music satisfy these criteria
"I find this conclusion both exciting and
"My second aesthetic claim in effect rejects this ["progressivist"] attitude in favor of the older view that music-making should be based on "nature". For the ancients, nature may have resided in the music of the spheres, but for us it lies in the musical mind."
== Reception ==
Lerdahl's paper has elicited many responses.
[[Roger Scruton]] praised it, calling it "hard-hitting".{{sfn|Scruton|2016}}
[[Nicholas Cook]] wrote, "The idea that music is a process of communication in which listeners decode structures that composers have encoded... is... based on several disputable assumptions: that people choose to listen grammatically; that there is, or ought to be, an equivalance between compositional and listening grammars; and, most fundamentally, that there is such a thing as musical grammar" ([[#Cook1994|Cook 1994, 88]]). He writes that Lerdahl▼
▲[[Nicholas Cook]] wrote, "The idea that music is a process of communication in which listeners decode structures that composers have encoded... is... based on several disputable assumptions: that people choose to listen grammatically; that there is, or ought to be, an
<blockquote>...assume(s) that there should be a more or less linear relationship between the manner in which a composer conceives a composition and the manner in which a listener perceives it. ...Lerdahl's aim is to specify the conditions that must be fulfilled if there is to be conformity between 'compositional grammar' and 'listening grammar'. And... he ends up by measuring existing music against the stipulations of his theory, using this as a basis for aesthetic evaluation. The result is to write off not only the Darmstadt avant-garde and minimalism, but also huge swathes of non-Western and popular music. ([[#Cook1999|Cook 1999, 241]])</blockquote>▼
▲<blockquote>
He asks:
<blockquote>What... does an article like Lerdahl's
Morag Josephine Grant wrote, "The paradox of Lerdahl's argument... is that while it is perfectly acceptable to adopt the composer's own system when dealing with compositional-technical analysis, it seems equally acceptable to revert to musical thinking of a quite different type when the aural result comes to be analysed" ([[#Grant2001|Grant 2001, 218]]). She continued, "Lerdahl's argument that musical language, like spoken language, is generative in structure excludes the possibility of other, non-hierarchical methods of achieving musical coherence... Lerdahl's concentration on the audibility of the row... blinds or deafens him to the simple fact that the use of the row is itself a constraint, not just on the composer, but in the aid of comprehensibility as well" ([[#Grant2001|Grant 2001, 219]]).▼
▲Morag Josephine Grant wrote, "The paradox of Lerdahl's argument... is that while it is perfectly acceptable to adopt the composer's own system when dealing with compositional-technical analysis, it seems equally acceptable to revert to musical thinking of a quite different type when the aural result comes to be analysed"
<blockquote>we have plenty of music that does not conform to Lerdahl's grammar: what, then, are people who claim to find it as interesting as tonal-metrical music actually doing? Either they are deluding themselves, or they are lying, or they have non-human brains. None of these answers seems entirely satisfactory. But if we do not like any of these answers, then we must admit that it is a matter of exposure and acquired understanding after all, in which case we are certainly a far cry from innate psychological universals ([[#Croft1999|Croft 1999, 54]]) [...] Vague language and tacit assumptions can be brought into the service of conservativism and aesthetic authoritarianism. It points to the misguided nature of attempts to turn the question of the dissemination of post-tonal music from an aesthetic, political, and indeed economic issue into a cognitive-scientific one. In this age when words like 'accessibility' and 'communication' are used too frequently and with too little understanding, it is of some significance that at least one major attempt to give scientific respectability to the conservative side of the debate fails ([[#Croft1999|Croft 1999, 55]]).</blockquote>▼
▲John Croft's master's degree thesis examines Lerdahl's essay in depth. In his conclusion, he wrote:<blockquote>we have plenty of music that does not conform to Lerdahl's grammar: what, then, are people who claim to find it as interesting as tonal-metrical music actually doing? Either they are deluding themselves, or they are lying, or they have non-human brains. None of these answers seems entirely satisfactory. But if we do not like any of these answers, then we must admit that it is a matter of exposure and acquired understanding after all, in which case we are certainly a far cry from innate psychological universals
For additional opinions and discussion, see
== References ==
▲For additional opinions and discussion, see [[#Bauer2004|Bauer 2004]], [[#Boros1995|Boros 1995]], [[#Boros1996|Boros 1996]], [[#Denham2009|Denham 2009]], [[#Dibben1996|Dibben 1996]], [[#Heinemann1993|Heinemann 1993]], [[#Heinemann1998|Heinemann 1998]], [[#Horn2015|Horn 2015]], and [[#Mosch2004|Mosch 2004]].
{{reflist|15em}}
'''Sources'''
{{div col|colwidth=45em}}
* {{cite journal|last=Boros|first=James|year=1995|title=A 'New Totality'?|journal=[[Perspectives of New Music]]|volume=33|number=1/2|pages=538–553}}
* {{cite journal|last=Boros|first=James|year=1996|title=A Response to Lerdahl|journal=Perspectives of New Music|volume=34|number=1|pages=252–258|doi=10.2307/833505 |jstor=833505 }}
* {{
* {{
* {{
* {{cite journal|last=Denham|first=A. E.|date=September 2009|title=The Future of Tonality|journal=[[British Journal of Aesthetics]]|volume=49 |issue=4 |pages=427–450|doi=10.1093/aesthj/ayp031 }}
* {{
* {{
* {{
* {{
* {{
* {{
{{div col end}}
==Further
{{div col|colwidth=45em}}
* {{wikicite |ref=Ashby2004 |reference=Ashby, Arved. 2004. "Intention and Meaning in Modernist Music." In ''The Pleasure of Modernist Music: Listening, Meaning, Intention, Ideology'', ed. Arved Ashby, 23-45. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.}}▼
*
▲*
*
*
▲* {{wikicite |ref=Bouz2013 |reference=Bouz, John. 2013. ''Examining the Application of Principles of Auditory Perception to Music Analysis: Building a Perception-based Music Theory to Capture Musical Features of Palestrina’s Missa Aeterna Christi Munera.'' A Thesis submitted for the degree of Master of Arts, Department of Music, University of Calgary.}}
*
▲* {{wikicite |ref=Cook1994 |reference=Cook, Nicholas. 1994. "Perception: A Perspective from Music Theory." In ''Musical Perceptions'', ed. Rita Aiello with John A. Sloboda, 64–95. Oxford: Oxford University Press.}}
* [[Fred Lerdahl|Lerdahl, Fred]] (1988). "Cognitive Constraints on Compositional Systems." In ''Generative Processes in Music: The Psychology of Performance, Improvisation, and Composition'', ed. [[John Sloboda]],
▲* {{wikicite |ref=Cook1999 |reference=Cook, Nicholas. 1999. "Analysing Performance and Performing Analysis." In ''Rethinking Music'', ed. Nicholas Cook and Mark Everist, 239–261. Oxford: Oxford University Press.}}
*
▲* {{wikicite |ref=Cook2007 |reference=Cook, Nicholas. 2007. ''Music, Performance, Meaning: Selected Essays.'' Ashgate Contemporary Thinkers on Critical Musicology Series. Aldershot: Ashgate.}}
*
▲* {{wikicite |ref=Croft1999 |reference=Croft, John. 1999. ''Musical memory, complexity, and Lerdahl’s cognitive constraints.'' A Thesis submitted for the degree of Master of Music, Department of Music, The University of Sheffield.}}
{{div col end}}
▲* {{wikicite |ref=Dibben1996 |reference=Dibben, Nicola. 1996. ''The Role of Reductional Representations in the Perception of Atonal Music.'' Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Department of Music, The University of Sheffield.}}
▲* {{wikicite |ref=Grant2001 |reference=Grant, Morag Josephine. 2001. ''Serial Music Serial Aesthetics: Compositional Theory in Post-War Europe.'' Music in the Twentieth Century, Arnold Whittall, general editor. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.}}
▲* {{wikicite |ref=Heinemann1993 |reference=Heinemann, Stephen. 1993. "Pitch-class Set Multiplication in Boulez’s "Le Marteau sans maitre" with [Original Composition]"". A Thesis submitted for the degree of Doctor of Musical Arts, Department of Music, University of WA.}}
▲* {{wikicite |ref=Heinemann1998 |reference=Heinemann, Stephen. 1998. "Pitch-Class Set Multiplication in Theory and Practice". ''Music Theory Spectrum'' 20, no. 1: 72–96.}}
▲* {{wikicite |ref=Horn2015 |reference=Horn, Walter. 2015. "Tonality, Musical Form, and Aesthetic Value". ''Perspectives of New Music'' 53, no. 2: 201-235.}}
▲*Lerdahl, Fred (1988). "Cognitive Constraints on Compositional Systems." In ''Generative Processes in Music: The Psychology of Performance, Improvisation, and Composition'', ed. John Sloboda, 231-59. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Reprinted in ''Contemporary Music Review'' 6/2 (1992), pp. 97–121. [http://www.bussigel.com/lerdahl/pdf/Cognitive%20Constraints%20on%20Compositional%20Systems.pdf]
▲* {{wikicite |ref=Meelberg2006 |reference=Meelberg, Vincent. 2006. ''New Sounds, New Stories: Narrativity in Contemporary Music.'' Leiden: Leiden University Press.}}
▲* {{wikicite |ref=Mosch2004 |reference=Mosch, Ulrich. 2004. ''Musikalisches Hören serieller Musik: Untersuchungen am Beispiel von Pierre Boulez’ «Le Marteau sans maître».'' Saarbrücken: Pfau-Verlag.}}
▲* {{wikicite |ref=Rowe1992 |reference=Rowe, Robert. 1992. ''Interactive Music Systems.'' Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.}}
▲* {{wikicite|ref=Scruton |reference=Scruton, Roger. 2016. "[http://www.futuresymphony.org/the-clothes-have-no-emperor The Clothes Have No Emperor]". Future Symphony Institute (accessed 12 December 2021).}}
[[Category:Music cognition]]
|