Content deleted Content added
m Bot: Migrating 1 interwiki links, now provided by Wikidata on d:q6486708 |
m →Verbal system: c/e |
||
(43 intermediate revisions by 26 users not shown) | |||
Line 1:
{{Short description|Linguistic theory of creole language innovations}}The '''language bioprogram theory''' or '''language bioprogram hypothesis'''<ref>See
▲The '''language bioprogram theory''' or '''language bioprogram hypothesis''' (LBH – ''see [[:wikt:bioprogram]]'') is a theory arguing that the structural similarities between different [[creole language]]s cannot be solely attributed to their [[superstratum|superstrate]] and [[substratum|substrate]] languages. As articulated mostly by [[Derek Bickerton]],<ref>See {{Harvcoltxt|Bickerton|1981}}, {{Harvcoltxt|Bickerton|1983}} {{Harvcoltxt|Bickerton|1984}}, {{Harvcoltxt|Bickerton|1988}}, and {{Harvcoltxt|Bickerton|1991}}</ref> [[creolization]] occurs when the linguistic exposure of children in a community consists solely of a highly unstructured [[pidgin]]; these children use their innate language capacity to transform the pidgin, which characteristically has high syntactic variability,<ref>{{Harvcoltxt|Bickerton|1983|p=116}}</ref> into a language with a highly structured grammar. As this capacity is universal, the grammars of these new languages have many similarities.
==Syntactic similarities==
By comparing [[Hawaiian
*Sentence structure: [[subject–verb–object]] word order, with similar mechanisms for using word order to apply [[Focus (linguistics)|focus]] to one of these constituents.
*Articles: [[definite article]] applied to specific and identified noun phrase, [[indefinite article]] applied to specific and newly
*TMA ([[tense–modality–aspect]]) systems
*distinction of realized and unrealized [[Complement (linguistics)|complements]]
*relativization and subject-copying
*negation
Line 17 ⟶ 16:
*passive equivalents
Having analyzed these features, he believed that he was able to characterize, at least partly, the properties of innate grammar.<ref name="Bickerton1983">{{Harvcoltxt|Bickerton|1983|p=122}}</ref> In his LBH, Bickerton defined very precisely what he considers to be a creole: a language that has arisen out of a prior pidgin that had not existed for more than a generation and among a population where, at most, 20% were speakers of the dominant language and where the remaining 80% were linguistically diverse.{{Citation needed|date=February 2008}}
Although this hypothesis has enjoyed much popularity, it has been criticized.{{Who|date=February 2008}} Bickerton in his LBH, defined very precisely what he considers to be a creole: a language that has arisen out of a prior pidgin that had not existed for more than a generation and among a population where, at most, 20% were speakers of the dominant language and where the remaining 80% were linguistically diverse.{{Citation needed|date=February 2008}} Such a definition excludes many languages that might be called creoles.{{Citation needed|date=February 2008}} Moreover, lack of historical data makes it often impossible to evaluate such claims. In addition, many of the creole languages that fit this definition do not display all the twelve features,{{Citation needed|date=February 2008}} while, according to {{Harvcoltxt|Mühlhäusler|1986}}, the left-out creoles often display more of them. Another problem, raised by {{Harvcoltxt|Mufwene|1986}}, is that if the same bioprogram was the starting point of all creoles, one must explain the differences between them, and language diversity in general, as the bioprogram is universal.▼
{| class="wikitable"
|-
!Child
!colspan=2|Creole
|-
Line 53 ⟶ 51:
==Verbal system==
The verb [[grammatical conjugation|conjugation]] is typically close to an ideal tense–modality–aspect pattern. In this system, the absence or presence of auxiliary [[verb]]s
{| class="wikitable"
Line 61 ⟶ 59:
!colspan="3" style="background:#A89A9A"|Nonstative
!colspan="3" style="background:#81BFBE"|Stative
|-
!
|'''Hawaiian Creole'''||'''Haitian Creole'''||'''Sranan'''||'''Hawaiian Creole'''||'''Haitian Creole'''||'''Sranan'''
Line 67 ⟶ 65:
!Base Form (he walked; he loves)
|he walk
|li mache
|a waka
|he love
|li
|a lobi
|-
!Nonpunctual (he is/was walking)
|he stay walk
|l ap mache
|a e waka
|style="background:silver"|
Line 83 ⟶ 81:
!Anterior (He had walked; he loved)
|he bin walk
|li
|a ben waka
|he bin love
|li
|a ben lobi
|-
!Anterior + Nonpunctual (he was/had been walking)
|he bin stay walk
|li
|a ben e waka
|style="background:silver"|
Line 99 ⟶ 97:
!Irreal (He will/would walk;he will/would love)
|he go walk
|l
|a sa waka
|he go love
|l
|a sa lobi
|-
!Irreal + Nonpunctual (he will/would be walking)
|he go stay walk
|
|a sa e waka
|style="background:silver"|
Line 115 ⟶ 113:
!Anterior + Irreal (he would have walked/loved)
|he bin go walk
|li t
|a ben sa waka
|he bin go love
|li t
|a ben sa lobi
|-
!Anterior + Irreal + Nonpunctual (he would have been walking)
|he bin go stay walk
|li
|a ben sa e waka
|style="background:silver"|
Line 132 ⟶ 130:
The above table demonstrates syntactic similarities of creole languages. Stative verbs are those that cannot form the [[nonpunctual aspect]]. According to Bickerton, all observed creole languages strictly follow a structure that has the anterior particle precede the irreal particle, and the irreal particle precede the nonpunctual particle, although in certain languages some compounded forms may be replaced by other constructions.
==Creole Prototype Theory==
[[John
#to use grammatical inflection via affixing,
#to develop productive, nontransparent derivational affixes, or
#to use tone to
==Proposed empirical study==
Bickerton proposed in 1976 an empirical test of his theory, which involved putting families speaking mutually unintelligible languages on a previously uninhabited island for three years. Federal funding for the test was obtained, but the experiment was cancelled over concerns that [[informed consent]] could not be obtained, given the breadth of unknown possible hazards of participation.<ref>[http://www.pri.org/
== Criticism ==
▲Bickerton proposed an empirical test of his theory, which involved putting families speaking mutually unintelligible languages on a previously uninhabited island for three years. Federal funding for the test was obtained, but the experiment was cancelled over concerns that [[informed consent]] could not be obtained, given the breadth of unknown possible hazards of participation.<ref>[http://www.pri.org/theworld/?q=node/25954 A linguistic island experiment.] The World, Public Radio International, 24 April 2009.</ref>
Several aspects of the LBH have attracted criticism. {{Harvcoltxt|Siegel|2007}} disputes some of Bickerton's claims about Hawai'i Creole, claiming that the linguistic input of the children was not impoverished, since it came from an expanded pidgin, not a rudimentary one. Siegel also claims the features of Hawai'i Creole are not that similar to other creoles and that the substrate languages (especially [[Cantonese]] and [[Portuguese language|Portuguese]]) were a significant source of grammatical features. Siegel also makes the point that Hawai'i Creole emerged over two generations, not one.
{{Review|paragraph|date=September 2021}}
▲
==See also==
Line 148 ⟶ 152:
*[[Origin of language]]
*[[Origin of speech]]
* [[Innateness hypothesis]]
==References==
Line 155 ⟶ 160:
*{{citation
|last=Bickerton
|first=
|authorlink=Derek Bickerton
|title=Roots of Language
|publisher=Karoma Publishers
|year=1981
|isbn=0-89720-044-6
|url-access=registration
|url=https://archive.org/details/rootsoflanguage0000bick
}}
*{{citation
Line 166 ⟶ 173:
|first= Derek
|authorlink=Derek Bickerton
|year=1983
|title=Creole Languages
|journal=Scientific American
Line 173 ⟶ 180:
|pages=116–122
|doi=10.1038/scientificamerican0783-116
|bibcode= 1983SciAm.249a.116B
}}
*{{citation
Line 182 ⟶ 190:
|journal=The Behavioral and Brain Sciences
|volume=7
|issue= 2
|pages= 173–188
|doi= 10.1017/S0140525X00044149
|s2cid= 144264276
}}
*{{citation
Line 203 ⟶ 215:
|journal=Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages
|volume=6
|pages=25–58
|doi=10.1075/jpcl.6.1.03bic
}}
* {{Cite book
|last=Hall
|first=Robert
|authorlink = Robert A. Hall Jr.
|year=1966
|title=Pidgin and Creole languages'
|url=https://archive.org/details/pidgincreolelang0000hall
|url-access=registration
|publisher = [[Cornell University Press]]
|___location = Ithaca
|isbn=9780801401732
|ref=RHall
}}
*{{citation
|last=McWhorter
|first=John H.
|authorlink=John McWhorter
|year=1992
|title=Substratal influence in Saramaccan serial verb construction
|journal=Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages
|volume=4
Line 222 ⟶ 248:
|year=1986
|chapter=The Universalist and Substrate Hypotheses Complement One Another
|title=Substrata versus
|place=Amsterdam
|publisher=Benjamins
Line 228 ⟶ 254:
* {{citation
|last=Mühlhäusler
|first=
|year=1986
|title=Pidgin and Creole linguistics
Line 235 ⟶ 261:
}}
*{{citation
|
|
|authorlink=Sarah Thomason
|last2=Kaufman
Line 246 ⟶ 272:
|publisher=University of California Press
|edition=first
}}
* {{Cite journal
|last=Siegel
|first=Jeff
|year=2007
|title=Recent evidence against the Language Bioprogram Hypothesis: The pivotal case of Hawai'i Creole
|journal=Studies in Language
|volume=31
|issue=1
|pages=51–88
|doi=10.1075/sl.31.1.03sie
}}
*{{citation
|last=Singler
|first=
|year=1986
|title=Short Note
|journal=Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages
|volume=1
}}
{{Use dmy dates|date=April 2019}}
{{authority control}}
[[Category:Pidgins and creoles]]
[[Category:Language acquisition]]
[[Category:Linguistic theories and hypotheses|Bioprogram theory]]
|