Content deleted Content added
added Category:Attention using HotCat |
m Open access bot: url-access updated in citation with #oabot. |
||
(12 intermediate revisions by 7 users not shown) | |||
Line 1:
{{Short description|Theory of human visual attention}}
'''Feature integration theory''' is a theory of [[attention]] developed in 1980 by [[Anne Treisman]] and
==Stages==
According to Treisman, the first stage of the feature integration theory is the preattentive stage. During this stage, different parts of the brain automatically gather information about basic features (colors, shape, movement) that are found in the visual field. The idea that features are automatically separated appears counterintuitive. However, we are not aware of this process because it occurs early in perceptual processing, before we become conscious of the object.
The second stage of feature integration theory is the focused attention stage, where a subject combines individual features of an object to perceive the whole object. Combining individual features of an object requires attention, and selecting that object occurs within a "master map" of locations. The master map of locations contains all the locations in which features have been detected, with each ___location in the master map having access to the multiple feature maps. These multiple feature maps, or sub-maps, contain a large storage base of features. Features such as color, shape, orientation, sound, and movement are stored in these sub-maps <ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Kristjánsson |first1=Árni |last2=Egeth |first2=Howard |date=2020-01-01 |title=How feature integration theory integrated cognitive psychology, neurophysiology, and psychophysics |journal=Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics |language=en |volume=82 |issue=1 |pages=7–23 |doi=10.3758/s13414-019-01803-7 |issn=1943-393X|doi-access=free |pmid=31290134 }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Chan |first1=Louis K. H. |last2=Hayward |first2=William G. |date=2009 |title=Feature integration theory revisited: Dissociating feature detection and attentional guidance in visual search. |url=http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0096-1523.35.1.119 |journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance |language=en |volume=35 |issue=1 |pages=119–132 |doi=10.1037/0096-1523.35.1.119 |pmid=19170475 |issn=1939-1277|url-access=subscription }}</ref>.When attention is focused at a particular ___location on the map, the features currently in that position are attended to and are stored in "object files". If the object is familiar, associations are made between the object and prior knowledge, which results in identification of that object. This top-down process, using prior knowledge to inform a current situation or decision, is paramount in either identifying or recognizing objects.<ref>{{Cite book |last1=Nobre |first1=Kia |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=mtXQAgAAQBAJ |title=The Oxford Handbook of Attention |last2=Kastner |first2=Sabine |date=2014 |publisher=OUP Oxford |isbn=978-0-19-967511-1 |language=en}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Chan |first1=Louis K. H. |last2=Hayward |first2=William G. |date=2009 |title=Feature integration theory revisited: Dissociating feature detection and attentional guidance in visual search. |url=http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0096-1523.35.1.119 |journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance |language=en |volume=35 |issue=1 |pages=119–132 |doi=10.1037/0096-1523.35.1.119 |pmid=19170475 |issn=1939-1277|url-access=subscription }}</ref> In support of this stage, researchers often refer to patients
[[File:FITstages.png|alt=The stages of feature integration theory|thumb|300px|The stages of feature integration theory]]
Line 13 ⟶ 14:
==Experiments==
<!-- Deleted image removed: [[File:fourshapesexp.png|thumb|alt=An example of four colored shapes and two black letters.|An example of the stimuli found in Treisman et al. (1982).]] -->To test the notion that attention plays a vital role in visual perception, Treisman and Schmidt (1982) designed an experiment to show that features may exist independently of one another early in processing. Participants were shown a picture involving four objects hidden by two black numbers. The display was flashed for one-fifth of a second followed by a random-dot masking field that appeared on screen to eliminate "any residual perception that might remain after the stimuli were turned off".<ref>Cognitive Psychology, E. Bruce Goldstein, P 105</ref> Participants were to report the black numbers they saw at each ___location where the shapes had previously been. The results of this experiment verified Treisman and Schmidt's hypothesis. In 18% of trials, participants reported seeing shapes "made up of a combination of features from two different stimuli",<ref>Cognitive Psychology, E. Bruce Goldstein, P 105</ref> even when the stimuli had great differences; this is often referred to as an [[illusory conjunction]]. Specifically, illusory conjunctions occur in various situations. For example, you may identify a passing person wearing a red shirt and yellow hat and very quickly transform him or her into one wearing a yellow shirt and red hat. The feature integration theory provides explanation for illusory conjunctions; because features exist independently of one another during early processing and are not associated with a specific object, they can easily be incorrectly combined both in laboratory settings, as well as in real life situations.<ref>Treisman, A. Cognitive Psychology 12, 97-136 (1980)</ref>
As previously mentioned, Balint's syndrome patients have provided support for the feature integration theory. Particularly, Research participant R.M.,
[[File:treismanshapes.png|thumb|alt=The stimuli resembling a carrot, lake and tire, respectively.|The stimuli resembling a carrot, lake and tire, respectively. Treisman et al.(1986).]]
Line 26 ⟶ 27:
==See also==
* [[Binding problem]]
* [[Visual search]]
Line 34 ⟶ 35:
==References==
* Anne Treisman and
* Anne Treisman and
* Anne Treisman and
* Anne Treisman (1988). "Features and objects: the fourteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture." ''Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology'', '''40A''', pp. 201–236.
*Anne Treisman and [[Nancy Kanwisher]] (1998). "Perceiving visually presented objects: recognition, awareness, and modularity." ''Current Opinion in Neurobiology'', '''8''', pp. 218–226.
* J. M. Wolfe (1994). "Guided Search 2.0: A revised model of visual search." ''Psychonomic Bulletin & Review'',
== External links ==
|