Content deleted Content added
Citation bot (talk | contribs) Add: s2cid, author pars. 1-1. Removed parameters. Some additions/deletions were actually parameter name changes. | You can use this bot yourself. Report bugs here. | Suggested by Wikiminds34 | Category:Heuristics | via #UCB_Category 35/78 |
Lindsay658 (talk | contribs) |
||
(29 intermediate revisions by 6 users not shown) | |||
Line 1:
{{short description|A dual process theory of persuasion}}
The '''heuristic-systematic model of information processing''' ('''HSM''') is a widely recognized model by [[Shelly Chaiken]] that attempts to explain how people receive and process [[Persuasion|persuasive]] messages.<ref name="Chaiken" /> The model states that individuals can process messages in one of two ways: [[heuristics in judgment and decision making|heuristically]] or systematically. Whereas systematic processing entails careful and deliberative processing of a message, heuristic processing entails the use of simplifying decision rules or ‘heuristics’ to quickly assess the message content. The guiding belief with this model is that individuals are more apt to minimize their use of cognitive resources (i.e., to rely on heuristics), thus affecting the intake and processing of messages.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A.|date=1989|title=Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context|journal=In J. S. Veleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended Thought|publisher=New York: Guilford|pages=212–252}}</ref> HSM predicts that processing type will influence the extent to which a person is persuaded or exhibits lasting attitude change. HSM is quite similar to the [[elaboration likelihood model]], or ELM. Both models were predominantly developed in the early to mid-1980s and share many of the same concepts and ideas.<ref name =alba>Albarracin, D., Johnson, B. T., & Zanna, M. P. (2005). The handbook of attitudes. Mahwah, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.</ref>▼
The '''heuristic-systematic model of information processing''' ('''HSM''') is a widely recognized{{Citation needed|date=June 2024|reason=the source at the end of the sentence is a book written by the creator of the model - not a reliable source when it comes to others' perceptions of the model}} model by [[Shelly Chaiken]] that attempts to explain how people receive and process [[Persuasion|persuasive]] messages.<ref name="Chaiken" />
▲The
HSM predicts that processing type will influence the extent to which a person is persuaded or exhibits lasting [[Attitude (psychology)|attitude]] change. HSM is quite similar to the [[elaboration likelihood model]], or ELM. Both models were predominantly developed in the early- to mid-1980s and share many of the same concepts and ideas.<ref name="alba">Albarracin, D., Johnson, B. T., & Zanna, M. P. (2005). The handbook of attitudes. Mahwah, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.</ref>
== History ==
Early research investigating how people process persuasive messaging focused mainly on cognitive theories and the way the [[mind]] processed each element of a message. One of the early guiding principles of underlying motivations of persuasive communications came from [[Leon Festinger]]
In 1953, [[Carl Hovland|Hovland]], [[Irving Janis|Janis]], and Kelley noted that a sense of "rightness" accompanies holding opinions similar to the opinions of others. In 1987, Holtz and Miller reaffirmed this line of thought by noting, "When other people are perceived to hold similar attitudes, one's confidence in the validity of one's own attitude is increased."<ref name =Petty>Petty, R.E. & Cacioppo, J.T. (1986), Communication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change. New York; Springer-Verlag</ref>
Another concept that contributed to the HSM was the [[sufficiency principle]]. This principle reflected widespread notions that people use limited [[Cognition|cognitive]] resources, or use an "economy-minded" approach to [[information processing (psychology)|information processing]] when presented with persuasive information. Based on this thought, early assumptions said people were at least partially guided by the "[[principle of least effort]]". This principle stated that in the interest of economy, the mind would often process with the least amount of effort (i.e., use a
The developer and main researcher of the HSM was
== Heuristic processing ==
[[Heuristic]] processing uses judgmental rules known as knowledge structures that are learned and stored in [[memory]].<ref name=Chen /> The heuristic approach offers an economic advantage by requiring minimal [[cognitive]] effort on the part of the recipient.<ref name =Chaiken>Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic Versus Systematic Information Processing and the Use of Source Versus Message Cues in Persuasion. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 39(5), 752-766. Retrieved from SocINDEX database.</ref> Heuristic processing is related to the concept of "[[satisficing]]."<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Simon|first=Herbert A.|date=1955|title=A Behavioral Model of Rational Choice|journal=The Quarterly Journal of Economics|volume=69|issue=1|pages=99–118|doi=10.2307/1884852|jstor=1884852}}</ref>
Heuristic processing is governed by availability, accessibility, and applicability. Availability refers to the knowledge structure, or heuristic, being stored in memory for future use. Accessibility of the heuristic applies to the ability to retrieve the memory for use. Applicability of the heuristic refers to the relevancy of the memory to the judgmental task.<ref name="Chen">Chen, S., Duckworth, K., & Chaiken, S. (1999). Motivated Heuristic and Systematic Processing. Psychological Inquiry, 10(1), 44. Retrieved from SocINDEX database</ref> Due to the use of knowledge structures, a person using heuristic information processing is likely to agree with messages delivered by experts, or messages that are endorsed by others, without fully processing the semantic content of the message.<ref name="Eagly">Eagly, A.H. & Chaiken, S. (1993). Process theories of attitude formation and change: The elaboration likelihood and heuristic-systematic models. In A.H. Eagly & S. Chaiken, (Eds.), The psychology of attitudes. Orlando: Harcourt Brace: pp. 303-350.</ref> In comparison to systematic processing, heuristic processing entails judging the [[Validity (logic)|validity]] of messages by relying more on accessible context information, such as the identity of the source or other non-content cues. Thus, heuristic views de-emphasize detailed information evaluation and focus on the role of simple rules or cognitive heuristics in mediating persuasion.<ref name="Chaiken" /><ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Gigerenzer|first1=Gerd|last2=Gaissmaier|first2=Wolfgang|date=2011-01-10|title=Heuristic Decision Making|journal=Annual Review of Psychology|language=en|volume=62|issue=1|pages=451–482|doi=10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145346|pmid=21126183|issn=0066-4308|url=http://edoc.mpg.de/564971|hdl=11858/00-001M-0000-0024-F16D-5|hdl-access=free}}</ref>
Individuals may be more likely to use heuristic processing when an issue is less personally important to them (they have low
== Systematic processing ==
Systematic processing involves comprehensive and analytic, cognitive processing of judgment-relevant information.<ref name=Chen /> The systematic approach values source [[Reliability (statistics)|reliability]] and message content, which may exert stronger impact on persuasion, when determining message validity.<ref name=Chaiken /> Judgments developed from systematic processing rely heavily on in-depth treatment of judgment-relevant information and respond accordingly to the [[Semantics|semantic]] content of the message.<ref name=Chen /> Recipients developing attitudes from a systematic basis exert considerable cognitive effort and actively attempt to comprehend and evaluate the message's arguments. When processing systematically, recipients also attempt to assess their validity as it relates to the message's conclusion.
== Choosing systematic or heuristic processing ==
Both heuristic and systematic processes may occur independently. It is also possible for both to occur simultaneously in an additive fashion or in a way that the judgmental implications of one process lend a [[bias]] nature to the other.<ref name=Chen /> The heuristic-systematic model includes the hypothesis that attitudes developed or changed by utilizing heuristic processing alone will likely be less stable, less resistant to [[Counterargument|counterarguments]], and will be less predictive of subsequent behavior than attitudes developed or changed utilizing systematic processing.<ref name=Chaiken />
Message recipients using heuristic processing may sometimes choose to accept message conclusions they would otherwise have rejected, or vice versa, had they invested more time and effort to scrutinize the message.<ref name=Chaiken />
[[Source credibility]] affects persuasion under conditions of low, but not high, issue-involvement and response-involvement.<ref name="Chaiken" />
When economic concerns are predominant, the recipient will likely use heuristic processing to form a
== Practical application ==
Research into information processing, especially in persuasive messaging, can be applied in [[advertising]]. For instance, HSM has been used in Internet [[Web page|webpage]] considerations.
Research into information processing, especially in persuasive messaging, has a natural application in advertising. For instance, HSM has been used in Internet webpage considerations. In a 2002 study by Wathen & Burkell, they proposed a theory that separated the evaluation process into distinct segments. In the theory, the process began with low-effort examinations of peripheral cues (e.g., appearance, design, organization, and source reputation) then continued to a more high-effort analysis of the content of the information source. The proposed research also drew on social psychological theories of dual-processing, which stated that information processing outcomes were the result of interaction between a fast, associative information-processing mode based on low-effort heuristics, and a slow, rule-based information processing mode based on high-effort systematic reasoning. Wathen and Burkell proposed (but did not test) that if an individual determines that an online source does not meet an appropriate level of credibility at any one stage, then he or she will leave the site without further evaluation. They theorized that this “easy to discard” behavior was indicative of information-rich environments, where the assumption is that many other potential sources of information exist, and spending too much time on any one source is potentially wasteful.<ref name="wathen">Wathen, C. N., & Burkell, J. (2002). Believe it or not: Factors influencing credibility on the Web. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 53(2), 134–144</ref>▼
▲
The HSM has also been applied in medical decision-making contexts. A 2004 study by [[Suzanne Chambers|Suzanne K. Steginga]], PhD, and Stefano Occhipinti, PhD, [[Queensland Cancer Fund]] and the School of Applied Psychology, [[Griffith University]], [[Queensland]], [[Australia]], investigated the utility of the heuristic-systematic processing model as a framework for the investigation of patient decision making. A total of 111 men diagnosed with localized prostate cancer were assessed using [[verbal protocol analysis]] and self-report measures. The results showed: "Most men (68%) preferred that decision making be shared equally between them and their doctor. Men's use of the expert opinion heuristic was related to men's verbal reports of decisional uncertainty and having a positive orientation to their doctor and medical care; a desire for greater involvement in decision making was predicted by a high internal locus of health control. Trends were observed for systematic information processing to increase when the heuristic strategy used was negatively [[affect (psychology)|affect]]-laden and when men were uncertain about the probabilities for cure and side effects. There was a trend for decreased systematic processing when the expert opinion heuristic was used. Findings were consistent with the heuristic-systematic processing model and suggest that this model has utility for future research in applied decision making about health issues.<ref name="app">{{cite journal|authors=Steginga, Suzanne K.; Occhipinti, Stefano|title=The Application of the Heuristic-Systematic Processing Model to Treatment Decision Making about Prostate Cancer|journal=Med Decis Making|year= 2004|volume=24|number=6|pages=573–583|doi=10.1177/0272989X04271044|pmid=15534339|s2cid=36170137}}</ref>▼
▲The HSM has also been applied in medical decision-making contexts. A 2004 study by [[Suzanne Chambers|Suzanne K. Steginga]], PhD, and Stefano Occhipinti, PhD, [[Queensland Cancer Fund]] and the School of Applied Psychology
== Direction of future research ==▼
▲== Direction of future research ==
Originally the heuristic-systematic model was developed to apply to "validity seeking" [[persuasion]] contexts in which peoples' primary motivation is to attain accurate attitudes that align with relevant facts.<ref name=Chaiken /><ref name=Eagly /> Chaiken assumes that the primary processing goal of accuracy-motivated recipients is to assess the validity of persuasive messages, and that both heuristic and systematic processing can serve this objective.<ref name=Eagly /> Other motives beyond the validity-seeking persuasion context were identified by Chaiken and colleagues (1989) who proposed an expanded model that posits two additional motives that heuristic and systematic processing can serve: defense-motivation and impression-motivation.▼
{{Copy edit|date=June 2024|for=tone}}
▲Originally the heuristic-systematic model was developed to apply to "validity seeking" [[persuasion]] contexts in which peoples' primary motivation is to attain [[accurate]] attitudes that align with relevant [[Fact|facts]].<ref name="Chaiken" /><ref name="Eagly" /> Chaiken assumes that the primary processing goal of accuracy-motivated recipients is to assess the validity of persuasive messages, and that both heuristic and systematic processing can serve this objective.<ref name="Eagly" /> Other motives beyond the validity-seeking persuasion context were identified by Chaiken and colleagues (1989) who proposed an expanded model that posits two additional motives that heuristic and systematic processing can serve: defense-motivation and impression-motivation.
* Defense-motivation is the desire to form or defend particular attitudinal positions.
Line 50 ⟶ 57:
== Criticisms ==
A major criticism of HSM is that the model closely relates to [[Elaboration likelihood model|ELM]], which is also a dual-processing model discussing two main paths to
In ELM, the central route is reflective and requires a willingness to process and think about the message.
HSM specifically examines validity seeking persuasion settings concerning people's motivations within the social environment.<ref name="Eagly" />
Major assumptions exist with both HSM and ELM, which is why both models have generated debate and are often misconstrued. Systematic processing assumes that persuasion has occurred via the recipient's understanding and cognitive elaboration of the persuasive argument.<ref name="Eagly" /> In addition, researchers hypothesize that systematic processing requires and uses cognitive capacity, while heuristic processing makes low cognitive demands.<ref name="Eagly" /> Furthermore, both HSM and ELM assume that "capacity and motivation are important determinants of systematic process" which results in biased modes of processing (p. 327).<ref name="Eagly" /> With heuristic processing, there is less need to process information and cognitively in comparison to systematic processing.
This leads to another similarity between HSM and ELM, as attitudes and opinions developed through heuristic processing will tend to be "less stable, less resistant to counter-propaganda, and less predictive of behavior" in comparison to attitudes and opinions formed through detailed information within systematic processing (p. 327).<ref name=Eagly />
HSM postulates that heuristic and systematic processing can each influence both "independent" and "interdependent" effects on decision making by occurring simultaneously (p. 328).<ref name=Eagly />
== See also ==
* {{annotated link|Attitude change}}
*
*[[Elaboration likelihood model]]▼
* {{annotated link|Cognitive-experiential self-theory|Cognitive-experiential self-theory ('''CEST''')}}
*[[Extended parallel process model]]▼
* {{annotated link|Dual process theory}}
*[[Transportation theory (psychology)|Extended transportation-imagery model]]▼
* {{annotated link|Dual process theory (moral psychology)}}
* {{annotated link|Heuristic (psychology)}}
* {{annotated link|Information processing (psychology)}}
* {{annotated link|Need for cognition}}
* {{annotated link|Persuasion}}
== References ==
Line 73 ⟶ 88:
[[Category:Attitude change]]
[[Category:Heuristics]]
[[Category:1980s neologisms]]
[[Category:Persuasion]]
[[Category:Psychological models]]
[[Category:Mental processes]]
|