Content deleted Content added
m Open access bot: hdl added to citation with #oabot. |
ShelfSkewed (talk | contribs) Dab links |
||
(12 intermediate revisions by 9 users not shown) | |||
Line 1:
{{Short description|Ease with which the brain processes information}}
In [[cognitive psychology]], '''processing fluency''' is the ease with which information is processed by the [[brain]]. It is commonly treated as a synonym for '''cognitive fluency''', a term used to describe the subjective experience of ease or difficulty associated with mental tasks. Processing fluency influences a range of [[Judgment|judgments]] and [[Decision-making|decisions]], including perceptions of [[truth]], [[interpersonal attraction|attractiveness]], [[knowledge by acquaintance|familiarity]], and [[confidence]].
Several subtypes of processing fluency have been identified. '''Perceptual fluency''' refers to the ease of processing [[sense|sensory]] stimuli, which can be affected by factors such as [[Visual perception|visual clarity]], [[Contrast (vision)|contrast]], or exposure duration. '''Retrieval fluency''' involves the ease with which information is accessed from [[memory]].<ref name="Alter2009" />
Higher fluency is often associated with more favorable evaluations, even when the ease of processing is unrelated to the content itself, a [[cognitive bias]] known as the [[fluency heuristic]].
==Research==
Research in cognitive neuroscience and psychology has shown that processing fluency influences different kinds of judgments. For instance, perceptual fluency can contribute to the experience of familiarity when fluent processing is attributed to the past. Repeating the presentation of a stimulus, also known as [[Priming (psychology)|priming]], is one method for enhancing fluency. Jacoby and Dallas in 1981 argued that items from past experience are processed more fluently.<ref name=Jacoby1981/> This becomes a learned experience throughout our lifetime such that fluent items can be attributed to the past. Therefore, people sometimes take fluency as an indication that a stimulus is familiar even though the sense of familiarity is false.<ref name=Whittlesea1993/> Perceptual fluency literature has been dominated with research that posits that fluency leads to familiarity. Behavioral measures of fluency do not have the temporal resolution to properly investigate the interaction between fluency and familiarity.
Further evidence has shown that artificial techniques can be used to trick people into believing they have encountered a stimulus previously. In one experiment,<ref>{{Cite journal|
Fluency and familiarity have been shown to lead to the mere exposure effect. Research has found that repetition of a stimulus can lead to fluent processing which leads to a feeling of liking.<ref>{{Cite journal|
Later research observed that high perceptual fluency increases the experience of [[positive affect]].<ref name=Reber1998/> Research with [[psychophysiological]] methods corroborated this positive effect on affective experience: easy-to-perceive stimuli were not only judged more positively but increased activation in the [[zygomaticus major muscle]], the so-called "smiling muscle".<ref name=Winkielman2001/> The notion that processing fluency is inherently positive led to the [[processing fluency theory of aesthetic pleasure]],<ref name=Reber2004/> and it has been used to explain people's negative reactions towards migrants, who appear to be more difficult to process than non-migrants.<ref name=Rubin2010/>
Research relating to processing fluency and product design has shown that when the form of a product is highly unusual, it becomes difficult to process and is viewed less favourably than fluent counterparts.<ref name=":2">{{Cite journal|last=Bloch|first=Peter H.|date=1995|title=Seeking the Ideal Form: Product Design and Consumer Response
Other studies have shown that when presenting people with a factual statement, manipulations that make the statement easier to mentally process—even totally nonsubstantive changes like writing it in a cleaner [[font]] or making it [[rhyme]] or simply repeating it—can alter judgment of the truth of the statement, along with evaluation of the [[intelligence]] of the statement's author.<ref name=Bennett2010/> This is called the "[[illusory truth effect|illusion of truth]] effect". Multiple studies have found that subjects were more likely to judge easy-to-read statements as true.<ref name=Reber1999/><ref name=Newman2012/><ref name=Waldman2014/> This means that perceived beauty and judged truth have a common underlying experience, namely processing fluency. Indeed, experiments showed that [[beauty]] is used as an indication for the correctness of mathematical solutions. This supports the idea that beauty is intuitively seen as truth.<ref name=Reber2008/> Processing fluency may be one of the foundations of [[Intuition (knowledge)|intuition]]<ref name=Topolinski2009/> and the [[Eureka effect|"Aha!" experience]].<ref name=Topolinski2010/><ref name="Wray2011"/>
The truth effect can be induced by colour differences in statements as well. In a study,<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Unkelbach|first=Christian|date=2007-01-01|title=Reversing the truth effect: Learning the interpretation of processing fluency in judgments of truth.
Fluency has been shown to affect judgements of humour. In one study,<ref name=":1">{{Cite journal|last=Topolinski|first=Sascha|date=2014-07-04|title=A processing fluency-account of funniness: Running gags and spoiling punchlines|journal=Cognition and Emotion|volume=28|issue=5|pages=811–820|doi=10.1080/02699931.2013.863180|issn=0269-9931|pmid=24320137|s2cid=25039892}}</ref> participants were presented with jokes which were in easy to read or hard to read fonts. Participants were asked to rate which jokes they believed were more humorous. Participants gave higher ratings to jokes in easy to read fonts. It has been predicted that the jokes in easy to read fonts feel fluent, as they are easier to pronounce and this results in higher ratings.<ref name=":1" /> The fluent processing has been misattributed to the humour of the statement.
As high processing fluency indicates that the interaction of a person with the environment goes smoothly,<ref name="Winkielman2003"/> a person does not need to pay particular attention to the environment. By contrast, low processing fluency means that there are problems in the interaction with the environment which requires more attention and an analytical processing style to solve the problem. Indeed, people process information more shallowly when processing fluency is high and employ an analytical thinking style when processing fluency is low.<ref name=Alter2007/><ref name=Song2008/>
Line 26 ⟶ 30:
==Applications==
Basic research on processing fluency has been applied to marketing,<ref name=Schwarz2004/> to business names, and to finance. For example, [[psychologists]] have determined that, during the week following their [[Initial
==See also==
Line 37 ⟶ 41:
{{Reflist|32em|refs=
<ref name=Alter2006>{{cite journal|
<ref name=Alter2007>{{cite journal|doi=10.1037/0096-3445.136.4.569|pmid=17999571|title=Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning|journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: General|volume=136|issue=4|pages=569–576|year=2007|last1=Alter|first1=Adam L.|last2=Oppenheimer|first2=Daniel M.|last3=Epley|first3=Nicholas|last4=Eyre|first4=Rebecca N.|url=http://pages.stern.nyu.edu/~aalter/intuitive.pdf|url-status=bot: unknown|
</ref>
<ref name=Alter2009>{{cite journal|url=http://pages.stern.nyu.edu/~aalter/tribes.pdf|doi=10.1177/1088868309341564|pmid=19638628|title=Uniting the Tribes of Fluency to Form a Metacognitive Nation|journal=Personality and Social Psychology Review|volume=13|issue=3|pages=219–235|year=2009|last1=Alter|first1=A. L.|last2=Oppenheimer|first2=D. M.|url-status=bot: unknown|
<ref name=Bennett2010>{{cite news |title=Easy=True: How "cognitive fluency" shapes what we believe, how we invest, and who will become a supermodel |url=http://archive.boston.com/bostonglobe/ideas/articles/2010/01/31/easy__true/?page=full |publisher=The Boston Globe |first=Drake |last=Bennett |date=January 31, 2010 |url-status=bot: unknown |
<ref name=Greifeneder2010>{{cite journal|doi=10.1177/1948550610368434|title=On Writing Legibly: Processing Fluency Systematically Biases Evaluations of Handwritten Material|journal=Social Psychological and Personality Science|volume=1|issue=3|pages=230–237|year=2010|last1=Greifeneder|first1=R.|last2=Alt|first2=A.|last3=Bottenberg|first3=K.|last4=Seele|first4=T.|last5=Zelt|first5=S.|last6=Wagener|first6=D.|s2cid=57062084}}</ref>
<ref name=Jacoby1981>{{cite journal|doi=10.1037/0096-3445.110.3.306|title=On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning|journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: General|volume=110|issue=3|pages=306–340|year=1981|last1=Jacoby|first1=Larry L.|last2=Dallas|first2=Mark}}</ref>
<ref name=Leynes2012>{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.10.004|pmid=23063967|title=Event-related potential (ERP) evidence for fluency-based recognition memory|journal=Neuropsychologia|volume=50|issue=14|pages=3240–3249|year=2012|last1=Leynes|first1=P. Andrew |last2=Zish|first2=Kevin|s2cid=24815127}}</ref>
<ref name=Newman2012>{{Cite journal|
<ref name=Reber1998>{{cite journal|doi=10.1111/1467-9280.00008|title=Effects of Perceptual Fluency on Affective Judgments|journal=Psychological Science|volume=9|pages=45–48|year=1998|last1=Reber|first1=R.|last2=Winkielman|first2=P.|last3=Schwarz|first3=N.|citeseerx=10.1.1.232.8868|s2cid=238063}}</ref>
<ref name=Reber1999>{{cite journal|doi=10.1006/ccog.1999.0386|pmid=10487787|title=Effects of Perceptual Fluency on Judgments of Truth|journal=Consciousness and Cognition|volume=8|issue=3|pages=338–342|year=1999|last1=Reber|first1=Rolf|last2=Schwarz|first2=Norbert|s2cid=2626302}}</ref>
<ref name=Reber2004>{{cite journal|doi=10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_3|pmid=15582859|title=Processing Fluency and Aesthetic Pleasure: Is Beauty in the Perceiver's Processing Experience?|journal=Personality and Social Psychology Review|volume=8|issue=4|pages=364–382|year=2004|last1=Reber|first1=Rolf|last2=Schwarz|first2=Norbert|last3=Winkielman|first3=Piotr|hdl=1956/594|s2cid=1868463|hdl-access=free}}</ref>
<ref name=Reber2008>{{cite journal|doi=10.3758/PBR.15.6.1174|pmid=19001586|title=The use of heuristics in intuitive mathematical judgment|journal=Psychonomic Bulletin & Review|volume=15|issue=6|pages=1174–1178|year=2008|last1=Reber|first1=Rolf|last2=Brun|first2=Morten|last3=Mitterndorfer|first3=Karoline|hdl=1956/2734|s2cid=5297500|url=https://bora.uib.no/bitstream/1956/2734/1/R365B.pdf|hdl-access=free}}</ref>
<ref name=Rubin2010>{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/j.jesp.2009.09.006|title=A processing fluency explanation of bias against migrants|journal=Journal of Experimental Social Psychology|volume=46|pages=21–28|year=2010|last1=Rubin|first1=Mark|last2=Paolini|first2=Stefania|last3=Crisp|first3=Richard J.|url=http://www.media-diversity.org/en/additional-files/documents/b-studies-reports/A_processing_fluency_explanation_of_bias_against_migrants.pdf|url-status=bot: unknown|
<ref name=Rugg2007>{{cite journal|doi=10.1016/j.tics.2007.04.004|pmid=17481940|title=Event-related potentials and recognition memory|journal=Trends in Cognitive Sciences|volume=11|issue=6|pages=251–257|year=2007|last1=Rugg|first1=Michael D.|last2=Curran|first2=Tim|s2cid=1249891}}</ref>
<ref name=Schwarz2004>{{cite journal|doi=10.1207/s15327663jcp1404_2|ssrn=532222|title=Metacognitive Experiences in Consumer Judgment and Decision Making|journal=Journal of Consumer Psychology|volume=14|issue=4|pages=332–348|year=2004|last1=Schwarz|first1=Norbert|hdl=2027.42/142285|hdl-access=free}}</ref>
Line 70 ⟶ 74:
<ref name=Song2008>{{cite journal|doi=10.1521/soco.2008.26.6.791|title=Fluency and the Detection of Misleading Questions: Low Processing Fluency Attenuates the Moses Illusion|journal=Social Cognition|volume=26|issue=6|pages=791–799|year=2008|last1=Song|first1=Hyunjin|last2=Schwarz|first2=Norbert}}</ref>
<ref name=Topolinski2009>{{cite journal|doi=10.1037/a0014678|pmid=19203169|title=The architecture of intuition: Fluency and affect determine intuitive judgments of semantic and visual coherence and judgments of grammaticality in artificial grammar learning|journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: General|volume=138|issue=1|pages=39–63|year=2009|last1=Topolinski|first1=Sascha|last2=Strack|first2=Fritz|url=http://soco.uni-koeln.de/files/Topolinski_Strack_2009_JEPG_architecture_of_intuition.pdf|url-status=bot: unknown|
<ref name=Topolinski2010>{{cite journal|doi=10.1177/0963721410388803|title=Gaining Insight into the "Aha" Experience|journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science|volume=19|issue=6|pages=402–405|year=2010|last1=Topolinski|first1=S.|last2=Reber|first2=R.|
<ref name=Waldman2014>{{Cite news|url=http://www.slate.com/articles/health_and_science/science/2014/09/truthiness_research_cognitive_biases_for_simple_clear_conservative_messages.html|title=The Science of Truthiness|last=Waldman|first=Katy|date=2014-09-03|newspaper=Slate|language=en-US|issn=1091-2339|access-date=2016-10-03}}</ref>
Line 80 ⟶ 84:
<ref name=Winkielman2001>{{cite journal|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.989|pmid=11761320|title=Mind at ease puts a smile on the face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation elicits positive affect|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|volume=81|issue=6|pages=989–1000|year=2001|last1=Winkielman|first1=Piotr|last2=Cacioppo|first2=John T.}}</ref>
<ref name="Winkielman2003">{{cite book|editor1-last=Musch|editor1-first=Jochen|editor2-last=Klauer|editor2-first=Karl C.|title=The Psychology of Evaluation: Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion|date=2003|publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers|___location=Mahwah, NJ|isbn=9781135640590|pages=189–217|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=t1h6AgAAQBAJ
<ref name="Wray2011">{{cite news|last1=Wray|first1=H.|title=Aha! The 23-Across Phenomenon|work=APS Observer|publisher=Association for Psychological Science|url=http://www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/publications/observer/2011/january-11/aha-the-23-across-phenomenon.html|volume=24|issue=1|date=January 2011|url-status=bot: unknown|
}}
==Further reading==
*{{cite book|last1=Winkielman|first1=P.|last2=Huber|first2=D.E.|last3=Kavanagh|first3=L.|last4=Schwarz|first4=N.|chapter=Fluency of consistency: When thoughts fit nicely and flow smoothly|editor1-last=Gawronski|editor1-first=Bertram|editor2-last=Strack|editor2-first=Fritz|title=Cognitive Consistency: A Fundamental Principle in Social Cognition|publisher=Guilford Press|isbn= 9781609189464|date=2012|pages=89–111|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=gfMytVxgOM0C&pg=PA89|
[[Category:Aesthetics]]
|