Content deleted Content added
Alter: pages. Add: pages, volume, year, title, isbn, journal, authors 1-2. Removed parameters. Formatted dashes. Some additions/deletions were parameter name changes. | Use this tool. Report bugs. | #UCB_Gadget |
Citation bot (talk | contribs) Removed URL that duplicated identifier. | Use this bot. Report bugs. | #UCB_CommandLine |
||
(22 intermediate revisions by 14 users not shown) | |||
Line 1:
{{Short description|Linguistics approach / theory}}
{{Linguistics}}
The '''
The term ''usage-based'' was coined by [[Ronald Langacker]] in 1987.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Serafima Gettys, Patricia Bayona, Rocío Rodríguez|title=From a Usage-Based Model to Usage-Based Instruction: Testing the theory|url=http://ijehd.cgrd.org/images/vol4no2/6.pdf|journal=International Journal of Education and Human Developments|volume=4|pages=50}}</ref> Usage-based models of language have become a significant new trend in linguistics since the early 2000s.<ref name="Mengden2014" /> Influential proponents of usage-based linguistics include [[Michael Tomasello]], [[Joan Bybee]] and [[Morten H. Christiansen|Morten Christiansen]]. Together with related approaches, such as [[construction grammar]], [[emergent grammar]], and language as a [[complex adaptive system]], usage-based linguistics belongs to the wider framework of [[evolutionary linguistics]]. It studies the lifespan of linguistic units (e.g. words, suffixes), arguing that they can survive language change through frequent usage or by participating in usage-based generalizations if their syntactic, semantic or pragmatic features overlap with other similar constructions.<ref name="Christiansen&Chater_2008">{{cite journal | last1=Christiansen | first1=Morten H. | last2=Chater | first2=Nick | date=2008 | title=Language as shaped by the brain | journal=Behavioral and Brain Sciences | volume= 31| issue=5 | pages=489–558 | doi=10.1017/S0140525X08004998 | pmid=18826669 | url=https://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/id/eprint/168484/1/download10.pdf | access-date=2020-12-22 }}</ref> There is disagreement as to whether the approach is different from [[memetics]] or essentially the same.<ref name="Blackmore_2008">{{cite journal | last=Blackmore | first=Susan | date=2008 | title=Memes shape brains shape memes | journal=Behavioral and Brain Sciences | volume= 31| issue=5 | pages=513 | doi=10.1017/S0140525X08005037 | url=https://www.academia.edu/3444108 | access-date=2020-12-22 }}</ref>
== Disciplinary roots ==
Line 7 ⟶ 12:
West Coast cognitive functionalism (WCCF) played a major role in the creation of the usage-based enterprise.
Firstly, a crucial point in WCCF was [[Eleanor Rosch]]’s paper on semantic categories in human cognition,<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Boyes-Braem |first1=P |last2=Johnson |first2=D |last3=Gray |first3=W. |last4=Mervis |first4=C.B.|last5=Rosch |first5=E. |title=Basic objects in natural categories |journal=Cognitive Psychology |date=1976}}</ref> which studied fuzzy semantic categories with central and peripheral concepts. Subsequently, [[Robin Lakoff]] (1987) applied these concepts to linguistic studies. For usage-based models of language, these discoveries legitimized interest in the peripheral phenomena and inspired the examination of the ontological status of the rules themselves.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Iverson, G.K. |last2=Corrigan, R.L. |first1=Lima, S.D. |title=The reality of linguistic rules |___location=Amsterdam |publisher=John Benjamins |date=1994}}</ref>
Secondly, WCCF focuses on the effects of social/ textual context and cognitive processes on human thought, instead of established systems and representations, which motivated the study of external sources in usage-based language research. For example, in analyzing the differences between the grammatical notions of subject vs. topic, Li and Thompson (1976), found that the repetition of certain topics by a [[speech community]] resulted in the surfacing and crystallization of formal properties into syntactic entities, namely the subject.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Givon |first1=T |chapter=From discourse to syntax: Grammar as a processing strategy |editor=T. Givón |title=Discourse and Syntax |volume=12 |pages=81–109 |___location=New York |publisher=Academic Press |date=1979b}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |last1=Givon |
Thirdly, the WCCF methodology of [[linguistic typology]]
'''Langacker’s Cognitive Grammar'''
The term ‘usage-based’ was coined by [[Ronald Langacker]] in 1987, while doing research on [[Cognitive Grammar]]. Langacker identified commonly recurring linguistic patterns (patterns such as those associated with Wh- fronting, subject-verb agreement, the use of present participles, etc.) and represented these supposed rule-governed behaviours on a hierarchical structure. The Cognitive Grammar model represented grammar, semantics and lexicon as associated processes that were laid on a continuum, which provided a theoretical framework that was significant in studying the usage-based conception of language.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Murkherjee |first1=J.|chapter=Corpus Data in a Usage-Based Cognitive Grammar |editor1=K. Aijmer |editor2=B. Altenberg |title=Advances in Corpus Linguistics: Papers from the 23rd International Conference on English Language Research on Computerized Corpora (ICAME 23) Göteborg 22-26 May 2002 |series=Language and Computers: Studies in Practical Linguistics |volume=49 |pages=85–100 |___location=Amsterdam |publisher=Rodopi|date=2004}}</ref> Consequently, a usage-based model accounts for these rule-governed language behaviours by providing a representational scheme that is entirely instance-based, and able to recognize and uniquely represent each familiar pattern, which occurs with varying strengths at different instances. His usage-based model draws on the cognitive psychology of schemata,<ref>{{cite book|last1=Schank |first1=R.C.|last2= Abelson |first2=R.P.|title=Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding: an inquiry into human knowledge structures|___location=Hillsdale, NJ |publisher=Erlbaum|date=1977}}</ref> which are flexible hierarchical structures that are able to accommodate the complexity of mental stimuli. Similarly, as humans perceive linguistic abstractions as multilayered, ranging from patterns that occur across whole utterances to those that occur in phonetic material, the usage-based model acknowledges the differing levels of granularity in speakers’ knowledge of their language.
'''
[[Joan Bybee|Bybee]]’s work<ref>{{cite book|last1=Bybee |first1=J. L.|title=Morphology: A study of the relation between meaning and form|___location=Amsterdam |publisher=John Benjamins|date=1985}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |last=Bybee |first=J. L.|title=Phonology and language use |___location=Cambridge, UK |publisher=Cambridge University Press|date=2001}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |last=Bybee |first=J. L.|title=Frequency of use and the organization of language |___location=New York |publisher=Oxford University Press|date=2006}}</ref><ref name=":0">{{cite book|last1=Bybee |first1=J. L.|last2= Perkins |first2= R.D.|last3= Pagliuca |first3=W.|title=The evolution of grammar: tense, aspect, and modality in the languages of the world|___location=Chicago |publisher=University of Chicago Press|date=1994}}</ref> greatly inspired the creation of usage-based models of language. Bybee’s model makes predictions about and explains synchronic, diachronic and typological patterns within languages, such as which variants will occur in which contexts, what forms they will take, and about their diachronic consequences. Using the linguistic phenomenon of splits (when a word starts to show subtle polysemy, and morphological possibilities for the originally single form ensue), Bybee proves that even irreducibly irregular word-forms are seen to be non-arbitrary when the context it occurs in is taken into consideration in the very representation of morphology. Simultaneously, she shows that even seemingly regular allomorphy is context-sensitive. Splits also aligns with the idea that linguistic forms cannot be studied as isolated entities, but rather in relation to the strength of their attachment to other entities.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Luce |
=== Schmid's Entrenchment-and-Conventionalization model ===
[[Hans-Jörg Schmid]]’s "Entrenchment-and-Conventionalization" Model offers a comprehensive recent summary approach to usage-based thinking.<ref>{{Cite book|last=Schmid|first=Hans-Jörg|title=The dynamics of the linguistic system : usage, conventionalization, and entrenchment|date=2020|isbn=978-0-19-254637-1|edition=First|___location=Oxford|oclc=1139239358}}</ref> In great detail and with reference to many sub-disciplines and concepts in linguistics he shows how usage mediates between entrenchment, the establishment of linguistic habits in individuals via repetition and associations, and conventionalization, a continuous feedback cycle which builds shared collective linguistic knowledge. All three components connect linguistic utterance types with their respective situative settings and extralinguistic associations.
== Frequency explanation ==
Advocates of usage-based linguistics including Joan Bybee and Martin Haspelmath argue that statistics of language usage depend on [[frequency]]. For instance, it is argued that the English verb ''tell'' always has two arguments ('tell something to someone') unlike the verb ''sell,'' which more frequently only has a direct object in actual language usage ('sell something'). It is hypothesized that such differences in the recurrence of the [[indirect object]] depend on statistical learning based on the language usage encountered by the individual. [[Jae Jung Song]] argues that the frequency explanation is circular—certain patterns are often used by people because they are frequent—and that the explanation of frequency issues must be found outside themselves.<ref name="Song_20122">{{cite book|last=Song|first=Jae Jung|title=Word Order|publisher=Cambridge University Press|year=2012|isbn=9780521872140}}</ref>
== Constructions: Form-meaning pairings<ref>{{cite web |last1=Bybee |first1=Joan L. |title=Usage-based Theory and Exemplar Representations of Constructions |url=https://www.oxfordhandbooks.com/view/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199544004.001.0001/oxfordhb-9780199544004-e-032 |website=Oxford Handbooks Online}}</ref>==
{{Main|Construction grammar}}
Constructions have direct pairing of form to meaning without intermediate structures, making them appropriate for usage-based models. The usage-based model adopts constructions as the basic unit of form-meaning correspondence.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Filmore |first1=Charles J. |chapter=The mechanisms of Construction Grammar |title=Proceedings of the 14th
From a [[
*It drives me crazy.
Line 30 ⟶ 41:
Adjectives shown here include crazy, mad, and up the wall, which are semantically related to the word drive. In exemplar models, the idea that memory for linguistic experience is similar to memory for other types of memories is proposed. Every token of linguistic experience impacts cognitive representation. And when stored representations are accessed, the representations change. Additionally, memory storage can store detailed information about processed tokens during linguistic experience, including form and context that these tokens were used. In this model, general categories and grammar units can emerge from linguistic experiences stored in memories, as exemplars are categorized by similarity to each other. Contiguous experiences such as meaning and acoustic shape are also recorded to be linked to each other.
'''Constructions as
By these means repeated sequences become more fluent. Within a chunk, sequential links are graded in strength based on the frequency of the chunk or perhaps the transitions between the elements of a chunk. A construction is a chunk even though it may contain schematic slots, that is, the elements of a chunk can be interrupted.
Memory storage requires links to connect idiomatic phrases together. In chunking, repeated sequences are represented together as units which can be accessed directly.<ref>{{cite journal |doi=10.1017/S0272263100014698 |title=Sequencing in SLA |year=1996 |last1=Ellis |first1=Nick C. |journal=Studies in Second Language Acquisition |volume=18 |pages=91–126 |hdl=2027.42/139863 |s2cid=17820745 |hdl-access=free }}</ref><ref>{{cite book |last1=Newell |first1=Allen |title=Unified Theories of Cognition |___location=Cambridge |publisher=MIT Press |date=1990 |isbn=9780674921016}}</ref> Through this, repeated sequences are more frequent. Sequential links are assessed in strength based on the frequency of the chunk or transitions between elements within a chunk. Additionally, the individual elements of a chunk can link to elements in other contexts. The example of ‘drive someone crazy’ forms a chunk, however items that compose it are not analyzable individually as words that occur elsewhere in cognitive representation. As chunks are used more frequently, words can lose their associations with exemplars of the same word. This is known as [[de-categorialization]].
== See also ==
Line 49 ⟶ 58:
[[Category:Linguistic theories and hypotheses]]
|