Levels of processing model: Difference between revisions

Content deleted Content added
FghIJklm (talk | contribs)
No edit summary
m General fixes & manual cleanup
 
(138 intermediate revisions by 88 users not shown)
Line 1:
{{Short description|Psychological model of memory}}
The '''levels-of-processing effect''', identified by [[Fergus I. M. Craik]] and Robert S. Lockhart in 1972, describes [[memory]] [[recollection|recall]] of [[Stimulus (physiology)|stimuli]] as a function of the depth of mental processing. A stimulus’s mental processing depth is determined by connections with preexisting memory, time spent processing the stimulus, cognitive effort, and sensory input mode. Depth of processing falls on a shallow to deep continuum. Shallow processing (e.g., processing based on [[phonemic]] and [[Orthography|orthographic]] components) leads to a fragile memory trace that is susceptible to rapid decay. Conversely, deep processing (e.g., [[Semantics|semantic]] processing) results in a more durable memory trace. This theory contradicts the multi-store [[Atkinson-Shiffrin memory model]] in its representation of memory strength as continuously variable.
The '''levels of processing model''', created by [[Fergus I. M. Craik]] and Robert S. Lockhart in 1972, describes memory [[recollection|recall]] of [[Stimulus (physiology)|stimuli]] as a function of the depth of mental processing, where deeper levels of processing produce more elaborate and stronger [[memory]] than more shallow levels of processing. Shallow processing (e.g., processing based on [[phonemic]] and [[Orthography|orthographic]] components) leads to a fragile memory trace that is susceptible to rapid decay. Conversely, deep processing (e.g., [[semantic processing]]) results in a more durable memory trace.<ref name="CL1972"/> There are three levels of processing in this model. Structural or visual processing involves remembering only the physical quality of the word (e.g. how the word is spelled and how letters look). Phonemic processing includes remembering the word by the way it sounds (e.g. the word tall rhymes with fall). Lastly, in semantic processing, individuals encode the meaning of the word with another word that is similar or has similar meaning. Once the word is perceived, the brain allows for a deeper processing.
 
This theory contradicts the multi-store [[Atkinson-Shiffrin memory model]] which represents memory strength as being continuously variable, the assumption being that rehearsal always improves [[long-term memory]]. They argued that rehearsal that consists simply of repeating previous analyses (maintenance rehearsal) does not enhance long-term memory.<ref>{{cite book|author=Eysenck, M.|year=2006|chapter=Learning and Long-term memory|title=Fundamentals of cognition|edition=Second|___location=Hove, England|publisher=Psychology Press}}</ref>
 
In a study from 1975 (Craik and [[Endel Tulving|Tulving]]) participants were given a list of 60 words. Each word was presented along with three questions. The participant had to answer one of them. Those three questions were in one of three categories. One category of questions was about how the word was presented visually ("Is the word shown in ''italics''?"). The second category of questions was about the phonemic qualities of the word ("Does the word begin with the sound 'bee'?"). The third category of questions was presented so that the reader was forced to think about the word within a certain context. ("Can you meet one in the street [a friend]"?) The result of this study showed that the words which contained deep processing (the latter) were remembered better.<ref>{{cite journal|author1=Craik, F. I.|author2=Tulving, E.|name-list-style=amp|year=1975|title=Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory|journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: General|volume=104|issue=3|pages=268|doi=10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.268}}</ref>
 
==Modifiers==
[[Familiarity heuristic|Familiarity]], [[transfer-appropriate processing]], the [[self-reference effect]], and the explicit nature of a stimulus modify the levels-of-processing effect by manipulating mental processing depth factors.
 
===Familiarity===
A stimulus will have a higher [[Recollection|recall]] value if it is highly compatible with preexisting semantic structures (Craik, 1972). According to [[semantic network]] theories, this is because such a stimulus will have many connections to other encoded memories, which are activated based on closeness in semantic network structure.<ref>{{Cite (journal|doi=10.3758/BF03210735 |journal=Psychonomic Bulletin & Review |last=Rhodes, |first=MG |author2=Anastasi JS |title=The effects of a levels-of-processing manipulation on false recall |year=2000). |volume=7 |issue=1 |pages=158–62 |pmid=10780030 |doi-access=free }}</ref> This activation increases cognitive analysis, increasing the strength of the memory representation. The familiarity modifier has been tested in [[implicit memory]] experiments, where subjects report false memories when presented with related stimuli.<ref>{{cite (journal|last=Toth, |first=JP |year=1996) |title=Conceptual automaticity in recognition memory: Levels-of-processing effects on familiarity |journal=Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology |url=https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1196-1961.50.1.123 |volume=50 |issue=1 |pmid=8653094 |pages=123–38 |doi=10.1037/1196-1961.50.1.123 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080124111249/http://findarticles.com/p/articles/mi_qa3690/is_199603/ai_n8735087 |archive-date=2008-01-24 |url-access=subscription }}</ref>
 
===Specificity of processing===
Specificity of processing describes the increased recall value of a stimulus when presented in the method with which it was inputinputted. For example, auditory stimuli (spoken words and sounds) have the highest recall value when spoken, and visual stimuli have the highest recall value when a subject is presented with images.<ref name = Vaidya2002>{{Cite journal| volume = 40| pages = 2136–2143| last = (Vaidya| etfirst al.,= CJ |author2=Zhao M |author3=Desmond JE |author4=Gabrieli JDE | title = Evidence for cortical encoding specificity in episodic memory: memory-induced re-activation of picture processing areas | journal = Neuropsychologia | year = 2002) | url = http://web.mit.edu/gabrieli-lab/Publications/2002/Vaidya.Neuropsy.2002.pdf In| lexicaldoi (word= 10.1016/S0028-based3932(02)00053-2 | pmid = 12208009| issue = 12 | s2cid = 17108548}}</ref> In writing tasks, words are recalled most effectively with semantic cues (asking for words with a particular meaning) if they are encoded semantically (self-generated by the subject as being related to a particular meaning). Words are recalled most effectively with data-driven cues (word completion) if they are read, rather than generated by a subject.<ref>{{Cite journal| volume = 15| issue = 4| pages = 657–668| last = (Blaxton| first = TA| title = Investigating dissociations among memory measures: Support for a transfer-appropriate processing framework| journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition | year = 1989)| url = http://www.colby.edu/psychology/ps341fa06/papers/Blaxton,%201989.pdf | doi = 10.1037/0278-7393.15.4.657 }}</ref>
 
===Self-reference effect===
Levels of processing have been an integral part of learning about memory. The self-reference effect describes the greater recall capacity for a particular stimulus if it is related semantically to the subject. This can be thought of as a corollary of the familiarity modifier, because stimuli specifically related to an event in a person’sperson's life will have widespread activation in that person’sperson's semantic network (Symons & Johnson, 1997).<ref>{{Cite Forjournal| example,volume the= recall121| valueissue of= a3| personalitypages trait= adjective371–394| islast higher= whenSymons| subjectsfirst are= askedCS|author2=Thompson whetherBT the| traittitle adjective= appliesThe toself-reference themeffect thanin whenmemory: askedA whethermeta-analysis trait| adjectivepmid has= a9136641 meaning| similarjournal to= anotherPsychological traitBulletin (Kelley| etyear al., 2002).= 1997
| url = http://digitalcommons.uconn.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1008&context=chip_docs
| doi = 10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.371 | format = pdf | url-access = subscription}}</ref> For example, the recall value of a personality trait adjective is higher when subjects are asked whether the trait adjective applies to them than when asked whether trait adjective has a meaning similar to another trait.<ref>{{Cite journal| volume = 14| issue = 5| pages = 785–794| last = Kelley| first = WM |author2=Macrae CN |author3=Wyland CL |author4=Caglar S |author5=Inati S |author6= Heatherton TF | title = Finding the Self? An Event-Related fMRI Study | pmid = 12167262 | journal = Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience| year = 2002| doi = 10.1162/08989290260138672| citeseerx = 10.1.1.522.2494| s2cid = 2917200}}</ref>
 
===Implicit memory and levels-of-processing===
Implicit memory tests, in contrast with explicit memory tests, measure the recall value of a particular stimulus based on later performance on stimulus-related tasks. During these tasks, the subject does not explicitly recall the stimulus, but the previous stimulus still effectsaffects performance.<ref>{{Cite journal | volume = 45 | pages = 1043–1056 | last = (Roediger, | first = HL | title = Implicit memory: Retention without remembering | journal = American Psychologist | year = 1990) | doi = 10.1037/0003-066X.45.9.1043 | pmid = 2221571 | issue = 9 }}</ref> For example, in a word-completion implicit memory task, if a subject reads a list containing the word “dog"dog", the subject provides this word more readily when asked for three-letter words beginning in “d"d". The levels-of-processing effect is only found for explicit memory tests. One study found that word completion tasks were unaffected by levels of semantic encodings achieved using three words with various levels of meaning in common.<ref>{{Cite journal | issn = 0002-9556 | volume = 102 | issue = 2 | pages = 151–181 | last = (Schacter &| first = DL |author2=McGlynn, SM | title = Implicit memory: Effects of elaboration depend on unitization | journal = The American Journal of Psychology | year = 1989) | doi = 10.2307/1422950 | jstor = 1422950 | s2cid = 31679776 }}</ref> Another found that typical level-of-processing effects are reversed in word completion tasks; subjects recalled pictures pairs more completely if they were shown a word representing a picture rather than asked to rate a picture for pleasantness (semantic encoding).<ref>{{Cite journal | volume = 18 | issue = 6 | pages = 1251–1269 | last = (Roediger, | first = HL |author2=Stadler, ML |author3=Weldon, &MS |author4=Riegler GL | title = Direct comparison of two implicit memory tests: word fragment and word stem completion | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition | year = 1992) | doi = 10.1037/0278-7393.18.6.1251 | pmid = 1447550 }}</ref> Typical level-of-processing theory would predict that picture encodings would create deeper processing than lexical encoding (see discussion of visual sensory modes below).
 
"Memory over the short term and the long term has been thought to differ in many ways in terms of capacity, the underlying neural substrates, and the types of processes that support performance."<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Rose | first1 = N. S. | last2 = Craik | first2 = F. M. | year = 2012 | title = A processing approach to the working memory/long-term memory distinction: Evidence from the levels-of-processing span task | url = https://openscholarship.wustl.edu/etd/300| journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition | volume = 38 | issue = 4| pages = 1019–1029 | doi = 10.1037/a0026976 | pmid = 22268911 | url-access = subscription }}</ref>
==Sensory Modes ==
 
Different sensory modes, by their nature, involve different depths of processing, generally producing higher recall value in certain senses than others. However, there is significant room for the modifiers mentioned earlier to affect levels-of-processing to be activated within each sensory mode.
====Long-term memory====
We especially remember information if we relate it to ourselves.
Damage to the hippocampus produces an inability to form or retrieve new long-term memories, but the ability to maintain and reproduce a small subset of information over the short term is typically preserved.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Baddeley | first1 = A. | last2 = Warrington | first2 = E. | year = 1970 | title = Amnesia and the distinction between long- and short-term memory | journal = Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior | volume = 9 | issue = 2| pages = 176–189 | doi = 10.1016/S0022-5371(70)80048-2 }}</ref>
 
==Sensory modes ==
Different sensory modes, by their nature, involve different depths of processing, generally producing higher recall value in certain senses than others. However, there is significant room for the modifiers mentioned earlier to affect levels-of-processing to be activated within each sensory mode.
 
===Vision===
[[Visual perception|Visual input]] creates the strongest recall value of all senses, and also allows the widest spectrum of levels-of-processing modifiers. It is also one of the most widely studied. Within visual studies, pictures have been shown to have a greater recall value than words – the [[picture superiority effect]]. However, semantic associations have the reverse effect in picture memories appear to be reversed to those in other memories. When logical details are stressed, rather than physical details, an image’simage's recall value becomes lower (Intraub & Nicklos, 1985).<ref>{{Cite Visualjournal word| memorizationissn is= subject0278-7393 to| thevolume standard= modifiers11 mentioned| earlierissue of= semantic2 connection,| andpages was= used284–98 for| Craiklast &= Lockhart’sIntraub original| work.first = CraikH &|author2=Nicklos Lockhart’sS participants in the learning task| viewedtitle a= seriesLevels of words on a computer screenprocessing and answeredpicture simplememory: yes/nothe questionsphysical aboutsuperiority thoseeffect| wordspmid (e.g.,= "Is3157769 the| wordjournal printed= in capital letters?"). The typesJournal of questionsExperimental thePsychology: participantsLearning, wereMemory, askedand toCognition answer| wereyear designed= to1985 affect| thedoi manner= in which the words were encoded into memory10.1037/0278-7393.11.2.284 Certain}}</ref> questions hadWhen participants encode thecomparing [[Orthographyorthography|orthographic]] aspects of the stimuli (e.g.capitalization, "Isletter theand word printedshape), inphonological capital(word letters?"sound). Otherand questionssemantic had(word participants encode themeaning) [[phonologicalEncoding (memory)|encoding]] properties ofcues, the stimulihighest (e.g.,levels "Doesof thisrecall wordwere rhymefound with "DOG"?").the Othermeanings questionsof hadthe participantswords, encodefollowed theby [[semantic]]their aspectssounds ofand finally the stimuliwritten (e.g.,and "Doesshape-based thecues wordwere fitfound into generate the followingleast sentenceability -to "Thestimulate ________recall.<ref walkedname="CL1972">{{cite intojournal the| house").last In= aCraik subsequent| memoryfirst test,= Craik &FIM |author2=Lockhart foundRS that participants| attendingyear to= the1972| physicaltitle features= Levels of theprocessing: wordsA hadframework the weakestfor memory trace,research participants| attendingdoi to= the10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X acoustic| propertiesvolume of= the11 words| hadissue a= moderately6 strong| memorypages trace,= and671–84 participants| attendingjournal to= semantic propertiesJournal of theVerbal wordsLearning had& theVerbal strongestBehavior trace.| s2cid = 14153362 }}</ref>
 
===Hearing===
Auditory stimuli follow conventional levels-of-processing rules, although are somewhat weaker in general [[Recollection|recall]] value when compared with vision. Some studies suggest that auditory weakness is only present for [[explicit memory]] (direct recall), rather than [[implicit memory]].<ref>{{Cite journal|doi=10.3758/BF03210786 |volume=4 |issue=1 |pages=130–133 (|last=Habib &|first=R |author2=Nyberg, L |title=Incidental retrieval processes influence explicit test performance with data-limited cues |journal=Psychonomic Bulletin & Review |year=1997). |doi-access=free }}</ref> When test subjects are presented with auditory versus visual word cues, they only perform worse on directed recall of a spoken word versus a seen word, and perform about equally on implicit free-association tests. Within auditory stimuli, semantic analysis produces the highest levels of recall ability for stimuli. Experiments suggest that levels-of-processing on the auditory level is directly correlated with neural activation.<ref (seename “Neural= Evidence”Fletcher>{{Cite below)journal | doi = 10.1093/brain/121.7.1239 | volume = 121 | issue = 7 | pages = 1239–1248 | last = (Fletcher, | first = PC |author2=Shallice, &T |author3=Dolan, RJ | title = The functional roles of prefrontal cortex in episodic memory. I. Encoding | journal = Brain | year = 1998) | url = http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/reprint/121/7/1239 | format = pdf
| pmid = 9679776 | doi-access = free | hdl = 21.11116/0000-0001-A1F6-3 | hdl-access = free }}</ref>
 
===Touch===
[[Touch|Tactile]] memory representations are similar in nature to visual representations, although there is not enough data to reliably compare the strength of the two kinds of stimuli. One study suggests that there is a difference in mental processing level due to innate differences between visual and tactile stimuli representations (Kavitha Srinivas, Greene, & Easton, 1997). In this study, subjects were presented with an object in both visual and tactile form (a subject is shown a sphere but cannot touch it, and later is given a similar sphere to only hold and not view). Subjects had more trouble identifying size difference in visual fields than using tactile feedback. A suggestion for the lower level of size processing in visual fields is that it results from the high variance in viewed object size due to perspective and distance.<ref>{{Cite journal
|doi=10.3758/BF03214345
|volume=4
|issue=4
|pages=535–540
|last1=Srinivas K
|first1=Kavitha
|author2=Greene AJ
|author3=Easton RD
|title=Visual and tactile memory for 2-D patterns: Effects of changes in size and lef-right orientation
|journal=Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
|year=1997
|doi-access=free
}}</ref> In this study, subjects were presented with an object in both visual and tactile form (a subject is shown a sphere but cannot touch it, and later is given a similar sphere to only hold and not view). Subjects had more trouble identifying size difference in visual fields than using tactile feedback. A suggestion for the lower level of size processing in visual fields is that it results from the high variance in viewed object size due to perspective and distance.
 
===Smell===
[[Odor]] memory is weaker than visual memory, achieving a successful identification rate of only 70-80% of visual memory.<ref>{{Cite journal | issn = 0033-2909 | volume = 109 | issue = 2 | pages = 242–51 | last = (Schab, | first = FR | title = Odor memory: taking stock | journal = Psychological Bulletin | year = 1991) | doi = 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.242 | pmid = 2034752 }}</ref> Levels-of-processing effects have been found within odor memory if subjects are asked to “visualize”"visualize" smells and associate them with a particular picture. Subjects who perform this task have a different recall value on explicit memory tests than subjects who memorize smells using self-chosen methods. The difference in recall value, however, depends on the subject, and the subject’ssubject's ability to form images from odors. Attributing verbal attributes to odors has similar effects. Semantic processing of odors (e.g. attributing the “mud”"mud" odor to “smell"smell like a puddle”puddle") has found to have the most positive effects on recall.
 
==Neural Evidenceevidence ==
Several brain imaging studies using [[positron emission tomography]] and [[functional magnetic resonance imaging]] techniques have shown that higher levels of processing [[correlate]] with more brain activity and activity in different parts of the brain than lower levels. For example, in a lexical analysis task, subjects showed activity in the [[prefrontal cortex|left inferior prefrontal cortex]] only when identifying whether the word represented a living or nonliving object, and not when identifying whether or not the word contained an “a” (Kapur et al"a"., 1994). Similarly, an auditory analysis task showed increased activation in the left inferior prefrontal cortex when subjects performed increasingly [[semantic]] word manipulations (Fletcher et al., 1998). Synaptic aspects of word recognition have been correlated with the [[Operculum<ref>{{Cite (brain)journal|left frontaldoi operculum]]= and the cortex lining the junction of the inferior frontal and inferior precentral sulcus (Friederici, Opitz, & von Cramon, 2000)10. The self-reference effect also has neural correlates with a region of the medial [[prefrontal cortex]], which was activated in an experiment where subjects analyzed the relevance of data to themselves (W1073/pnas. M91. Kelley et al6.,2008| 2006).volume Specificity= of91| processingissue is= explained6| onpages a neurological basis by studies that show brain activity in the same ___location when a visual memory is encoded and retrieved, and lexical memory in a different ___location (Vaidya, Zhao, Desmond, & Gabrieli, 2002). Visual memory areas were mostly located within the bilateral [[Extrastriate= cortex2008–2011|extrastriate visuallast cortex]].= Kapur
| first = S|author2=FIM Craik |author3=E Tulving |author4=AA Wilson |author5=S Houle |author6=GM Brown | title = Neuroanatomical Correlates of Encoding in Episodic Memory: Levels of Processing Effect | journal = [[Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences]] | year = 1994 | pmid = 8134340| pmc = 43298 | doi-access = free| bibcode = 1994PNAS...91.2008K}}</ref> Similarly, an auditory analysis task showed increased activation in the left inferior prefrontal cortex when subjects performed increasingly [[semantic]] word manipulations.<ref name = Fletcher/> Synaptic aspects of word recognition have been correlated with the [[Operculum (brain)|left frontal operculum]] and the cortex lining the junction of the inferior frontal and inferior precentral sulcus.<ref>{{Cite journal | doi = 10.1093/cercor/10.7.698 | volume = 10 | issue = 7
| pages = 698–705 | last = Friederici | first = AD |author2=Opitz B |author3=Yves von Cramon D | title = Segregating semantic and syntactic aspects of processing in the human brain: an fMRI investigation of different word types | journal = Cereb. Cortex | year = 2000 | url = http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/reprint/10/7/698 | format = pdf | pmid = 10906316| doi-access = free | hdl = 11858/00-001M-0000-0010-D753-7 | hdl-access = free }}</ref> The self-reference effect also has neural correlates with a region of the medial [[prefrontal cortex]], which was activated in an experiment where subjects analyzed the relevance of data to themselves.<ref>{{Cite journal | last = Kelley | first = WM |author2=Macrae CN |author3=Wyland CL |author4=Caglar S |author5=Inati S |author6= Heatherton TF | title = Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study |year = 2002 | doi = 10.1162/08989290260138672 | pages = 785–794 | volume = 14 | issue = 5 | journal = Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | pmid = 12167262 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.522.2494 | s2cid = 2917200 }}</ref> Specificity of processing is explained on a neurological basis by studies that show brain activity in the same ___location when a visual memory is encoded and retrieved, and lexical memory in a different ___location.<ref name = Vaidya2002/> Visual memory areas were mostly located within the bilateral [[Extrastriate cortex|extrastriate visual cortex]].
 
==Mental Disordersdisorders==
Levels-of-processing effects interact in various ways with [[mental disorders]]. In particular, levels-of-processing effects appear to be strengthened in patients with [[Memory and aging|age-related memory degradation]], selectively strengthened in [[panic disorder]] patients, unaffected in [[Alzheimer's disease]] patients, and reversed in [[autistic]] patients.
 
===Age-Relatedrelated Memorymemory Degradationdegradation===
{{main|Memory and aging}}
Memory encoding strength derived from higher levels-of-processing appears to be conserved despite other losses in memory function with age. Several studies show that, in older individuals, the ability to process semantically in contrast with non-semantically is improved by this disparity (Grady & F. I. Craik, 2000). Neural imagining studies show decreased [[Prefrontal cortex|left-prefrontal cortex]] activity when words and images are presented to older subjects than with younger subjects, but roughly equal activity when assessing [[semantic]] connections (Grady & F. I. Craik, 2000).
Memory encoding strength derived from higher levels-of-processing appears to be conserved despite other losses in memory function with age. Several studies show that, in older individuals, the ability to process semantically in contrast with non-semantically is improved by this disparity. Neural imaging studies show decreased [[Prefrontal cortex|left-prefrontal cortex]] activity when words and images are presented to older subjects than with younger subjects, but roughly equal activity when assessing semantic connections.<ref>{{Cite journal | volume = 10 | issue = 2 | pages = 224–231 | last = Grady | first = CL |author2=Craik FIM | title = Changes in memory processing with age | journal = Current Opinion in Neurobiology |year = 2000 | doi = 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00073-8 | pmid = 10753795 | s2cid = 9567476 }}</ref>
 
===Panic Disordersdisorders===
Panic disorders appear to modify levels-of-processing by increasing ability to [[Recollection|recall]] words with [[Threat|threatening]] meanings over positive and neutral words. In one study, both implicit (free recall) and explicit (memory of emotional aspects) memorization of word lists were enhanced by threatening meanings in such patients.<ref>{{Cite (Cloitrejournal| &doi Liebowitz,= 1991)10.1007/BF01173032| Onevolume possible= interpretation15| ofissue this= is5| thatpages subjects= with371–386| last = Cloitre | first = M |author2=Liebowitz MR | title = Memory bias in panic disordersdisorder: processAn threateninginvestigation informationof morethe completelycognitive avoidance hypothesis | journal = Cognitive Therapy and immediately.Research| year = 1991 | s2cid = 24229675}}</ref>
 
===Alzheimer’sAlzheimer's Diseasedisease===
Modern studies show an increased effect of levels-of-processing in Alzheimer patients. Specifically, there is a significantly higher recall value for semantically encoded stimuli over physically encoded stimuli. In one such experiment, subjects maintained a higher recall value in words chosen by meaning over words selected by numerical order.<ref>{{Cite journal| volume = 6| issue = 8| pages = 583–588| last = (Scott,| first = L. C.|author2=G. K. Wright, |author3=G. S. Rai, |author4=A. N. Exton-Smith, &|author5=J. M. Gardiner, | title = Further evidence of preserved memory function in Alzheimer's disease| journal = International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry| year = 1991)| doi = 10.1002/gps.930060806| s2cid = 144360572}}</ref>
 
===Autism===
In autistic patients, levels-of-processing effects are reversed in that semantically presented stimuli have a lower recall value than physically presented stimuli. In one study, [[phonological]] and [[orthography|orthographic]] processing created higher recall value in word list-recall tests.<ref>{{Cite journal| volume = 40| issue = 7| pages = 964–969| last = (Toichi| &first = M |author2=Kamio, Y | title = Long-term memory and levels-of-processing in autism| journal = Neuropsychologia | year = 2002) | doi = 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00163-4| pmid = 11900748 | s2cid = 37972435}}</ref> Other studies have explicitly found non-semantically processed stimuli to be more accurately processed by autistic patients than in healthynon-autistic patients.<ref>{{Cite journal| doi = 10.1093/brain/awh561| volume = 128| issue = 10| pages = 2430–2441| last = (Bertone,| first = A |author2=Mottron, L |author3=Jelenic, &P |author4=Faubert, J | title = Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity| journal = Brain | date = 2005)-10-01| url = http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/128/10/2430| format = abstract| pmid = 15958508| doi-access = | url-access = subscription}}</ref> No clear conclusions have been drawn as to the cause of this oddity.
 
==References==
{{reflist}}
*{{Cite web
| last = Toth
| first = Jeffrey P
| title = Conceptual automaticity in recognition memory: Levels-of-processing effects on familiarity
| accessdate = 2007-11-17
| date = 1996-03-01
| url = http://findarticles.com/p/articles/mi_qa3690/is_199603/ai_n8735087
}}
 
*{{Cite web
| last = Matthew G. Rhodes
| first = Jeffery S. Anastasi
| title = The effects of a levels-of-procesing manipulation on false recall
| accessdate = 2007-11-17
| date = 2000
| url = http://lamar.colostate.edu/~mrhodes/RA00.pdf
}}
 
*{{Cite web
| last = F. I. M. Craik
| first = R. S. Lockhart
| title = Levels of Processing: A Framework for Memory Research
| accessdate = 2007-11-17
| date = 1972
| url = http://picard.montclair.edu/psychology/adams/craik-and-lockhart-1972.htm
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| volume = 15
| issue = 4
| pages = 657-668
| last = Blaxton
| first = Terasa A.
| title = Investigating Dissociations Among Memory Measures: Support for a Transfer-Appropriate Processing Framework
| journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition
| accessdate = 2007-11-17
| date = 1989
| url = http://www.colby.edu/psychology/ps341fa06/papers/Blaxton,%201989.pdf
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| volume = 40
| pages = 2136 - 2143
| last = Vaidya
| first = Chandan J.
| coauthors = Margaret Zhao, John E. Desmond, John D. E. Gabrieli
| title = Evidence for cortical encoding specificity in episodic memory: memory-induced re-activation of picture processing areas
| journal = Neuropsychologia
| accessdate = 2007-11-17
| date = 2002
| url = http://web.mit.edu/gabrieli-lab/Publications/2002/Vaidya.Neuropsy.2002.pdf
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| volume = 121
| issue = 3
| pages = 371-394
| last = Symons
| first = Cynthia S.
| coauthors = Blair T. Johnson
| title = The Self-Reference Effect in Memory: A Meta-Analysis
| journal = Psychological Bulletin
| accessdate = 2007-11-17
| date = 1997
| url = http://digitalcommons.uconn.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1008&context=chip_docs
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| issn = 00029556
| volume = 102
| issue = 2
| pages = 151-181
| last = Schacter
| first = Daniel L
| coauthors = Susan M McGlynn
| title = Implicit Memory: Effects of Elaboration Depend on Unitization
| journal = The American Journal of Psychology
| accessdate = 2007-11-17
| date = 1989
| url = http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0002-9556%28198922%29102%3A2%3C151%3AIMEOED%3E2.0.CO%3B2-H
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| volume = 5
| issue = 1-2
| pages = 142-164
| last = Challis
| first = Bradford H.
| coauthors = Boris M. Velichkovsky, Fergus I. M. Craik
| title = Levels-of-Processing Effects on a Variety of Memory Tasks: New Findings and Theoretical Implications
| journal = Consciousness and Cognition
| accessdate = 2007-11-18
| date = 1996-03
| url = http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6WD0-45MGTW0-9/2/9f7803d8be5bea23e924bc850f9dd100
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| issn = 02787393
| volume = 11
| issue = 2
| pages = 284-98
| last = Intraub
| first = H
| coauthors = S Nicklos
| title = Levels of processing and picture memory: the physical superiority effect
| journal = Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition
| date = 1985-04
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| doi = 10.1093/brain/121.7.1239
| volume = 121
| issue = 7
| pages = 1239-1248
| last = Fletcher
| first = PC
| coauthors = T Shallice, RJ Dolan
| title = The functional roles of prefrontal cortex in episodic memory. I. Encoding
| journal = Brain
| accessdate = 2007-11-18
| date = 1998-07-01
| url = http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/121/7/1239
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| volume = 10
| issue = 5/6
| pages = 319-332
| last = Roediger III
| first = Henry L.
| coauthors = David A. Gallo, Lia Geraci
| title = Processing approaches to cognition: The impetus from the levels-of-processing framework
| journal = Memory
| accessdate = 2007-11-18
| date = 2002
| url = http://www.wjh.harvard.edu/~dgallo/Roedigeretal02.pdf
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| volume = 4
| issue = 1
| pages = 130-133
| last = Habib
| first = Reza
| coauthors = Lars Nyberg
| title = Incidental retrieval processes influence explicit test performance with data-limited cues
| journal = Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
| accessdate = 2007-11-18
| date = 1997
| url = http://www.psychonomic.org/search/view.cgi?id=821
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| volume = 4
| issue = 4
| pages = 535-540
| last = Kavitha Srinivas
| coauthors = Anthony J. Greene, Randolph D. Easton
| title = Visual and tactile memory for 2-D patterns: Effects of changes in size and lef-right orientation
| journal = Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
| accessdate = 2007-11-18
| date = 1997
| url = http://www.psychonomic.org/search/view.cgi?id=195
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| issn = 00332909
| volume = 109
| issue = 2
| pages = 242-51
| last = Schab
| first = F R
| title = Odor memory: taking stock
| journal = Psychological bulletin
| date = 1991-03
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| doi = 10.1073/pnas.91.6.2008
| volume = 91
| issue = 6
| pages = 2008-2011
| last = Kapur
| first = S
| coauthors = FIM Craik, E Tulving, AA Wilson, S Houle, GM Brown
| title = Neuroanatomical Correlates of Encoding in Episodic Memory: Levels of Processing Effect
| journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
| accessdate = 2007-11-18
| date = 1994-03-15
| url = http://www.pnas.org/cgi/content/abstract/91/6/2008
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| doi = 10.1093/cercor/10.7.698
| volume = 10
| issue = 7
| pages = 698-705
| last = Friederici
| first = Angela D.
| coauthors = Bertram Opitz, D. Yves von Cramon
| title = Segregating Semantic and Syntactic Aspects of Processing in the Human Brain: an fMRI Investigation of Different Word Types
| journal = Cereb. Cortex
| accessdate = 2007-11-18
| date = 2000-07-01
| url = http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/10/7/698
}}
 
*{{Cite web
| last = W. M. Kelley
| coauthors = C. N. Macrae, C. L. Wyland, S. Caglar, S. Inati, T. F. Heatherton
| title = Finding the Self? An Event-Related fMRI Study
| format = research-article
| accessdate = 2007-11-18
| date = 2006-03-13
| url = http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/abs/10.1162/08989290260138672
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| volume = 14
| issue = 5
| pages = 785-794
| last = Kelley
| first = W. M.
| coauthors = C. N. Macrae, C. L. Wyland, S. Caglar, S. Inati, T. F. Heatherton
| title = Finding the Self? An Event-Related fMRI Study
| journal = Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
| date = 2002
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| volume = 45
| pages = 1043-1056
| last = Roediger
| first = H. L.
| title = Implicit memory
| journal = American Psychologist
| date = 1990
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| volume = 18
| issue = 6
| pages = 1251-1269
| last = ROEDIGER
| first = HL
| coauthors = ML STADLER, MS WELDON, GL RIEGLER
| title = Direct comparison of two implicit memory tests: word fragment and word stem completion
| journal = Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition
| date = 1992
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| volume = 6
| issue = 8
| pages = 583-588
| last = Scott
| first = L. C.
| coauthors = G. K. Wright, G. S. Rai, A. N. Exton-Smith, J. M. Gardiner
| title = Further evidence of preserved memory function in Alzheimer's disease
| journal = International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry
| accessdate = 2007-12-06
| date = 1991
| url = http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/gps.930060806
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| volume = 10
| issue = 2
| pages = 224-231
| last = Grady
| first = Cheryl L
| coauthors = Fergus IM Craik
| title = Changes in memory processing with age
| journal = Current Opinion in Neurobiology
| accessdate = 2007-12-06
| date = 2000-04-01
| url = http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6VS3-402CMNM-D/2/5a710c1a36f3f449843952a8d4361743
}}
 
*{{Cite journal
| doi = 10.1007/BF01173032
| volume = 15
| issue = 5
| pages = 371-386
| last = Cloitre
| coauthors = Liebowitz
| title = Memory bias in panic disorder: An investigation of the cognitive avoidance hypothesis
| journal = Cognitive Therapy and Research
| accessdate = 2007-12-06
| date = 1991-10-01
| url = http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01173032
}}
 
{{memory}}
*{{Cite journal
| volume = 40
| issue = 7
| pages = 964-969
| last = Toichi
| first = Motomi
| coauthors = Yoko Kamio
| title = Long-term memory and levels-of-processing in autism
| journal = Neuropsychologia
| accessdate = 2007-12-06
| date = 2002
| url = http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/B6T0D-45BCXJ9-10/2/43a1f4a6ecb33938538b8572349b197a
}}
 
[[Category:Memory]]
*{{Cite journal
[[Category:Memory biases]]
| doi = 10.1093/brain/awh561
| volume = 128
| issue = 10
| pages = 2430-2441
| last = Bertone
| first = Armando
| coauthors = Laurent Mottron, Patricia Jelenic, Jocelyn Faubert
| title = Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity
| journal = Brain
| accessdate = 2007-12-06
| date = 2005-10-01
| url = http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/128/10/2430
}}