Language family: Difference between revisions

[pending revision][accepted revision]
Content deleted Content added
 
Line 1:
{{short description|Group of languages related through a common ancestor}}
[[Image:Human_Language_Families_Map_%28Wikipedia_Colors_.PNG|thumb|right|380px|Current distribution of Human Language Families]]
{{pp-pc|small=yes}}
Most [[language]]s are known to belong to '''language families''' ("families" hereforth). An accurately identified family is a [[phylogeny|phylogenetic]] unit, that is, all its members derive from a common ancestor. This ancestor is very seldom known to us directly, since most languages have a very short recorded history. However, it is possible to recover many of its features by applying the [[comparative method]] — a reconstructive procedure worked out by [[19th-century]] [[linguist]] [[August Schleicher]]. This can demonstrate the validity of many of the proposed families listed below.
{{Use dmy dates|date=July 2017}}
[[File:Primary Human Languages Improved Version.png|upright=1.8|thumb|2005 map of the contemporary distribution of the world's primary language families]]
 
A '''language family''' is a group of [[language]]s related through descent from a common ancestor, called the [[proto-language]] of that family. The term ''family'' is a metaphor borrowed from biology, with the [[tree model]] used in [[historical linguistics]] analogous to a [[family tree]], or to [[phylogenetic tree]]s of taxa used in evolutionary [[taxonomy (biology)|taxonomy]]. Linguists thus describe the ''daughter languages'' within a language family as being ''genetically related''.<ref>{{cite book|first1=Bruce M. |last1=Rowe |first2=Diane P. |last2=Levine |title=A Concise Introduction to Linguistics |year=2015 |publisher=Routledge |isbn=978-1-317-34928-0 |pages=340–341 |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=ePQ5CgAAQBAJ&pg=PA340 |access-date=26 January 2017}}</ref> The divergence of a proto-language into daughter languages typically occurs through geographical separation, with different [[regional dialect]]s of the proto-language undergoing different [[language change]]s and thus becoming distinct languages over time.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Dimmendaal |first1=Gerrit J. |title=Historical Linguistics and the Comparative Study of African Languages |year=2011 |publisher=John Benjamins |isbn=978-9-027-28722-9 |page=336 |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=e-PxyCpnnzEC&pg=PA336 |access-date=26 January 2017}}</ref>
Language families can be divided into smaller phylogenetic units, conventionally referred to as ''branches'' of the family, because the history of a language family is often represented as a [[tree diagram]]. However, the term ''family'' is not restricted to any one level of this "tree"; the [[Germanic languages|Germanic]] family, for example, is a branch of the [[Indo-European]] family. Some taxonomists do restrict the term ''family'' to a certain level, but there is little consensus in how to do this. Those who do affix such labels also subdivide branches into ''groups'', and groups into ''complexes''. They also aggregate families into ''phyla'' (also known as ''stocks'', or ''superfamilies''). Phyla are often used to aggregate American Indian language families. One method for doing all of this is called [[glottochronology]].
 
One well-known example of a language family is the [[Romance languages]], including [[Spanish language|Spanish]], [[French language|French]], [[Italian language|Italian]], [[Portuguese language|Portuguese]], [[Romanian language|Romanian]], [[Catalan language|Catalan]], [[Romansh language|Romansh]], and many others, all of which are descended from [[Vulgar Latin]].{{refn|group=note|[[Vernacular]] [[Latin]], as opposed to the [[Classical Latin]] used as a literary language.}}<ref name="ethnologue">Lewis, M. Paul, Gary F. Simons, and Charles D. Fennig (eds.). [http://www.ethnologue.com ''Ethnologue: Languages of the World''], Seventeenth edition. Dallas, Texas: SIL International, 2013.</ref> The Romance family itself is part of the larger [[Indo-European]] family, which includes many other languages native to [[Europe]] and [[South Asia]], all believed to have descended from a common ancestor known as [[Proto-Indo-European]].
The common ancestor of a family is known as its ''[[protolanguage]]''. For example, the reconstructible [[protolanguage]] of the well-known Indo-European family is called [[Proto-Indo-European]]. This is not known from written records, since it was spoken before the invention of writing, but sometimes a protolanguage can be identified with a historically known language. Thus, provincial dialects of [[Latin]] ("[[Vulgar Latin]]") gave rise to the modern [[Romance language]]s, so the Proto-Romance language is more or less identical with Latin (if not exactly with the literary Latin of the Classical writers), and dialects of [[Old Norse]] are the protolanguage to [[Norwegian language|Norwegian]], [[Swedish language|Swedish]], [[Danish language|Danish]], [[Faroese language|Faroese]] and [[Icelandic language|Icelandic]].
 
A language family is usually said to contain at least two languages, although [[language isolate]]s — languages that are not related to any other language — are occasionally referred to as families that contain one language. Conversely, there is no upper bound to the number of languages a family can contain. Some families, such as the [[Austronesian languages]], contain over 1000.<ref>{{cite web |title=Family: Austronesian|url=https://glottolog.org/resource/languoid/id/aust1307 |website=Glottolog 5.0 |access-date=3 August 2024}}</ref>
Languages that cannot be reliably classified into any family are known as [[language isolate]]s. A language isolated in its own branch within a family, such as [[Greek language|Greek]] within Indo-European, is often also called an isolate, but such cases are usually clarified. For instance, Greek might be referred to as an Indo-European isolate.
 
Language families can be identified from characteristics shared amongst their languages. [[Sound change]]s are one of the strongest pieces of evidence that can be used to identify a genetic relationship because of their predictable and consistent nature, and through the [[comparative method]] can be used to reconstruct proto-languages. However, languages can also change through [[language contact]], which can falsely suggest genetic relationships. For example, the [[Mongolic languages|Mongolic]], [[Tungusic languages|Tungusic]], and [[Turkic languages]] share many similarities that have led several scholars to believe [[Altaic languages|they were related]]. These supposed relationships were later discovered (in the view of most scholars) to be derived through language contact and thus they are not related through shared ancestry.<ref>{{cite journal |last=De la Fuente |first=José Andrés Alonso |year=2016 |title=Review of Robbeets, Martine (2015): Diachrony of verb morphology. Japanese and the Transeurasian languages |url=https://www.academia.edu/30240029 |journal=Diachronica |volume=33 |issue=4 |pages=530–537 |doi=10.1075/dia.33.4.04alo}}</ref> Eventually though, intense language contact with other language families, and inconsistent changes within the original language family, will obscure inherited characteristics and make it virtually impossible to deduce earlier relationships; even the oldest demonstrable language family, [[Afroasiatic languages|Afroasiatic]], is far younger than language itself.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Boë |first1=Louis-Jean |display-authors=etal |title=Which way to the dawn of speech?: Reanalyzing half a century of debates and data in light of speech science |journal=Science |date=11 December 2019 |volume=5 |issue=12 |doi=10.1126/sciadv.aaw3916 |pmc=7000245 }}</ref>
==Largest families==
 
==Major language families==
According to the numbers in [[Ethnologue]][http://www.ethnologue.com/web.asp], the largest language families in terms of number of languages are:
{{Main|List of language families}}
Estimates of the number of language families in the world may vary widely. According to ''[[Ethnologue]]'' there are 7,151 [[living language|living human language]]s across 142 different language families.<ref>{{cite web|title=How many languages are there in the world?|url=https://www.ethnologue.com/guides/how-many-languages |date=3 May 2016 |website=[[Ethnologue]] |language=en |access-date=26 March 2021}}</ref><ref name=":1">{{cite web|title=What are the largest language families? |url=https://www.ethnologue.com/guides/largest-families |date=25 May 2019 |website=[[Ethnologue]] |language=en |access-date=3 March 2020}}</ref> [[Lyle Campbell]] (2019) identifies a total of 406 independent language families, including isolates.<ref name="Campbell 2019">{{cite journal |last=Campbell |first=Lyle |title=How many language families are there in the world? |journal=Anuario del Seminario de Filología Vasca "Julio de Urquijo" |publisher=UPV/EHU Press |volume=52 |issue=1/2 |date=2019-01-08 |issn=2444-2992 |doi=10.1387/asju.20195 |page=133|s2cid=166394477 |doi-access=free |hdl=10810/49565 |hdl-access=free }}</ref>
 
''[[Ethnologue]]'' 27 (2024) lists the following families that contain at least 1% of the 7,164 known languages in the world;<ref name="families24">{{Cite web |date=2024-04-13 |title=Welcome to the 24th edition |url=https://www.ethnologue.com/browse/families/ |website=Ethnologue |language=en}}</ref> while ''[[Glottolog]]'' 5.0 (2024) lists the following as the largest families, of 7,788 languages (other than [[Sign language|sign languages]], [[Pidgin|pidgins]], and [[Unclassified language|unclassifiable languages]]):<ref>{{Cite web |title=Glottolog 5.0 - |url=https://glottolog.org/glottolog/family |access-date=2023-06-25 |website=glottolog.org}}</ref>
# [[Niger-Congo]] (1514 languages)
{{table alignment}}
# [[Austronesian]] (1268 languages)
{|
# [[Trans-New Guinea]] (564 languages) [validity disputed]
|valign=top|
# [[Indo-European]] (449 languages)
{|class="wikitable col2right"
# [[Sino-Tibetan]] (403 languages)
|+Ethnologue 27
# [[Afro-Asiatic]] (375 languages)
# [[Nilo-Saharan]] (204!Family!!No. of<br>languages)
|-
# [[Pama-Nyungan languages|Pama-Nyungan]] (178 languages)
|[[Niger-Congo languages|Niger–Congo]] ||1,552
# [[Oto-Manguean]] (174 languages)
|-
# [[Austro-Asiatic]] (169 languages)
|[[Austronesian languages|Austronesian]]|| 1,256
# [[Sepik-Ramu]] (100 languages) [validity disputed]
|-
# [[Tai-Kadai]] (76 languages)
# |[[Tupi]]Trans–New (76Guinea languages)|Trans–New Guinea]]|| 481
|-
# [[Dravidian languages|Dravidian]] (73 languages)
# |[[MayanSino-Tibetan languages|MayanSino-Tibetan]]|| (69 languages)458
|-
|[[Indo-European languages|Indo-European]]|| 454
|-
|[[Australian Aboriginal languages|Australian]]|| 384
|-
|[[Afroasiatic languages|Afroasiatic]]||382
|-
|[[Nilo-Saharan languages|Nilo-Saharan]]|| 210
|-
|[[Oto-Manguean languages|Otomanguean]]|| 179
|-
|[[Austroasiatic languages|Austroasiatic]]|| 167
|-
|[[Kra-Dai languages|Kra-Dai]]|| 91
|-
|[[Dravidian languages|Dravidian]] ||85
|}
|width=20|
|valign=top|
{|class="wikitable col2right"
|+Glottolog 5.0
!Family!!No. of<br>languages
|-
| [[Atlantic–Congo languages|Atlantic–Congo]]|| 1,410
|-
| [[Austronesian languages|Austronesian]]|| 1,274
|-
| [[Indo-European languages|Indo-European]]|| 586
|-
| [[Sino-Tibetan languages|Sino-Tibetan]]|| 514
|-
| [[Afro-Asiatic languages|Afroasiatic]] ||381
|-
| [[Trans–New Guinea languages|Trans–New Guinea]] ||316
|-
| [[Pama-Nyungan languages|Pama–Nyungan]] ||250
|-
| [[Oto-Manguean languages|Otomanguean]]|| 181
|-
| [[Austroasiatic languages|Austroasiatic]]|| 158
|-
| [[Tai-Kadai languages|Tai–Kadai]]|| 95
|-
| [[Dravidian languages|Dravidian]]|| 85
|-
| [[Arawakan languages|Arawakan]] ||77
|}
|}
Language counts can vary significantly depending on what is considered merely a dialect; for example [[Lyle Campbell]] counts only 27 Otomanguean languages, although he, ''Ethnologue'' and ''Glottolog'' also disagree as to which languages belong in the family.
 
Neither Ethnologue<ref>{{Cite web |title=Sign Language |url=https://www.ethnologue.com/subgroup/2/ |access-date=2025-03-15 |website=ethnologue.com}}</ref> nor Glottolog<ref>{{Cite web |title=Pseudo Family: Sign Language |url=https://glottolog.org/resource/languoid/id/sign1238 |access-date=2025-03-15 |website=glottolog.org}}</ref> list the language families of [[sign language]]s. Of sign language families, the [[Francosign languages|Francosign family]], hosting around 70 languages, is by far the largest and most widespread.
== Language families (spoken) ==
 
The largest five language families in terms of number of speakers (Indo-European, Sino-Tibetan, Afro-Asiatic, Niger-Congo and Austronesian) make up five-sixths (almost 83.3%) of the world’s population.<ref name=":1" />
In the following, each "bulleted" item is a known language family. The geographic headings over them are meant solely as a tool for grouping families into collections more comprehensible than an unstructured list of the dozen or two of independent families. Geographic relationship is convenient for that purpose, but these headings are ''not'' a suggestion of any "super-families" phylogenetically relating the families named.
 
== Genetic relationship ==
=== [[Africa]] and [[southwest Asia]] ===
Two languages have a ''genetic relationship'', and belong to the same language family, if both are descended from a common ancestor through the process of [[language change]], or one is descended from the other.
:'''''See main article, [[African languages]]'''''
The term and the process of language evolution are independent of, and not reliant on, the terminology, understanding, and theories related to [[genetics]] in the biological sense, so, to avoid confusion, some linguists prefer the term ''genealogical relationship''.<ref>{{Cite journal| volume=28| issue=1| pages=209–223| last=Haspelmath| first=Martin |author-link=Martin Haspelmath| title=How hopeless is genealogical linguistics, and how advanced is areal linguistics? — Review of Aikhenvald & Dixon (2001): Areal diffusion and genetic inheritance| journal=Studies in Language| date=2004-05-05| doi=10.1075/sl.28.1.10has| url=https://zenodo.org/record/580172}} p. 222.</ref><ref name="francois" />{{rp|184}}
 
There is a remarkably similar pattern shown by the linguistic tree and the genetic tree of human ancestry<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Henn |first1=B. M. |last2=Cavalli-Sforza |first2=L. L. |author-link2=Luigi Luca Cavalli-Sforza |last3=Feldman |first3=M. W. |author-link3=Marcus Feldman |title=The great human expansion |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences |date=17 October 2012 |volume=109 |issue=44 |pages=17758–17764 |doi=10.1073/pnas.1212380109 |pmc=3497766 |bibcode=2012PNAS..10917758H |pmid=23077256 |jstor=41829755 |doi-access=free}}</ref>
# [[Afro-Asiatic languages|Afro-Asiatic (Hamito-Semitic) languages]]
that was verified statistically.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Cavalli-Sforza |first1=L. L. |author-link=Luigi Luca Cavalli-Sforza |last2=Minch |first2=E. |last3=Mountain |first3=J. L. |title=Coevolution of genes and languages revisited |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America |date=15 June 1992 |volume=89 |issue=12 |pages=5620–5624 |pmid=1608971 |pmc=49344 |bibcode=1992PNAS...89.5620C |doi=10.1073/pnas.89.12.5620 |jstor=2359705 |doi-access=free}}</ref> Languages interpreted in terms of the putative phylogenetic tree of human languages are transmitted to a great extent vertically (by ancestry) as opposed to horizontally (by spatial diffusion).<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Gell-Mann |first1=M. |author-link=Murray Gell-Mann |last2=Ruhlen |first2=M. |author-link2=Merritt Ruhlen |title=The origin and evolution of word order |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences |date=10 October 2011 |volume=108 |issue=42 |pages=17290–17295 |doi=10.1073/pnas.1113716108 |bibcode=2011PNAS..10817290G |jstor=41352497 |url=http://authors.library.caltech.edu/59840/1/17290.full.pdf |pmid=21987807 |pmc=3198322 |doi-access=free}}</ref>
# [[Niger-Congo languages]]
# [[Nilo-Saharan languages]]
# [[Khoisan languages]]
 
===Establishment===
=== [[Europe]], and [[North Asia|north]], [[West Asia|west]], and [[south Asia]] ===
{{main|Comparative method}}
# [[Indo-European languages]]
In some cases, the shared derivation of a group of related languages from a common ancestor is directly [[attested language|attested]] in the historical record. For example, this is the case for the [[Romance languages|Romance language family]], wherein [[Spanish language|Spanish]], [[Italian language|Italian]], [[Portuguese language|Portuguese]], [[Romanian language|Romanian]], and [[French language|French]] are all descended from Latin, as well as for the [[North Germanic languages|North Germanic]] language family, including [[Danish language|Danish]], [[Swedish language|Swedish]], [[Norwegian language|Norwegian]] and [[Icelandic language|Icelandic]], which have shared descent from [[Proto-Norse language|Ancient Norse]]. Latin and ancient Norse are both attested in written records, as are many intermediate stages between those ancestral languages and their modern descendants.
# [[Dravidian languages]] (some include '''Dravidian''' languages in a larger [[Elamo-Dravidian languages|Elamo-Dravidian language]] family.)
# [[Caucasian languages]] (generally thought to be two separate families, [[North Caucasian languages|North Caucasian]] and [[South Caucasian languages|Kartvelian]])
# [[Altaic languages]] (disputed)
# [[Uralic languages]]
# [[Hurro-Urartian languages]] ([[extinct language|extinct]])
# [[Yukaghir languages]] (Some include Yukaghir in the '''Uralic''' family.)
# [[Chukotko-Kamchatkan languages]]
# [[Yeniseian languages]]
# [[Andamanese languages]] (two families)
 
In other cases, genetic relationships between languages are not directly attested. For instance, the Romance languages and the North Germanic languages are also related to each other, being subfamilies of the [[Indo-European language family]], since both Latin and Old Norse are believed to be descended from an even more ancient language, [[Proto-Indo-European]]; however, no direct evidence of Proto-Indo-European or its divergence into its descendant languages survives. In cases such as these, genetic relationships are established through use of the [[comparative method]] of linguistic analysis.
=== [[East Asia|East]] and [[Southeast Asia]] and [[the Pacific]] ===
# [[Australian Aboriginal languages]] (multiple unrelated families)
# [[Austroasiatic languages]]
# [[Austronesian languages|Austronesian (Malayo-Polynesian) languages]]
# [[Hmong-Mien languages]]
# [[Japonic languages]] (or [[Fuyu languages]])
# [[Papuan languages]] (multiple unrelated families)
# [[Sino-Tibetan languages]]
# [[Tai-Kadai languages]]
# Shahedul Haque, NSU
 
In order to test the hypothesis that two languages are related, the comparative method begins with the collection of pairs of words that are hypothesized to be [[cognate]]s: i.e., words in related languages that are derived from the same word in the shared ancestral language. Pairs of words that have similar pronunciations and meanings in the two languages are often good candidates for hypothetical cognates. The researcher must rule out the possibility that the two words are similar merely due to chance, or due to one having [[Loanword|borrowed]] the words from the other (or from a language related to the other). Chance resemblance is ruled out by the existence of large collections of pairs of words between the two languages showing similar patterns of phonetic similarity. Once coincidental similarity and [[Loanword|borrowing]] have been eliminated as possible explanations for similarities in sound and meaning of words, the remaining explanation is common origin: it is inferred that the similarities occurred due to descent from a common ancestor, and the words are actually cognates, implying the languages must be related.<ref>{{cite book|last=Campbell|first=Lyle|title=Historical Linguistics|date=2013|publisher=MIT Press}}</ref>
=== [[North America]] ===
[[Image:Langs_N.Amer.png|thumb|Distribution of language families and isolates north of Mexico at first contact.]]
: '''''See main article, [[Native American languages]]'''''
# [[Algic languages]] (incl. [[Algonquian languages]]) (29)
# [[Alsean languages]] (2)
# [[Caddoan languages]] (5)
# [[Chimakuan languages]] (2)
# [[Chinookan|Chinookan languages]] (3)
# [[Chumashan languages]] (6)
# [[Comecrudan languages]] (3)
# [[Coosan languages]] (2)
# [[Eskimo-Aleut languages]] (7)
# [[Guacurian languages]] (a.k.a. Waikurian) (8)
# [[Iroquoian languages]] (11)
# [[Kalapuya|Kalapuyan languages]] (3)
# [[Kiowa-Tanoan languages]] (7)
# [[Maidu|Maiduan languages]] (4)
# [[Mayan languages]] ([[North America]] & [[Central America]]) (31)
# [[Muskogean languages]] (6)
# [[Na-Dené languages]] (40)
# [[Oto-Manguean languages]] ([[North America]] & [[Central America]]) (27)
# [[Palaihnihan languages]] (2)
# [[Plateau Penutian languages]] (a.k.a. Shahapwailutan) (4)
# [[Pomoan languages]] (7)
# [[Salishan languages]] (23)
# [[Shastan languages]] (4)
# [[Siouan languages]] (16)
# [[Tequistlatecan languages]] (3)
# [[Totonacan languages]] (2)
# [[Tsimshian|Tsimishian languages]] (2)
# [[Utian languages]] (12)
# [[Uto-Aztecan languages]] (31)
# [[Wakashan languages]] (6)
# [[Wintu|Wintuan languages]] (4)
# [[Yokutsan languages]] (3)
# [[Yukian languages]] (2)
# [[Yuman-Cochimí languages]] (11)
 
====Linguistic interference and borrowing====
=== [[Central America]] and [[South America]] ===
When languages are in [[Language contact|contact with one another]], either of them may influence the other through [[Language transfer#Broader effects of language transfer|linguistic interference]] such as borrowing. For example, [[French language|French]] has influenced [[English language|English]], [[Arabic language|Arabic]] has influenced [[Persian language|Persian]], [[Sanskrit]] has influenced [[Tamil language|Tamil]], and [[Chinese language|Chinese]] has influenced [[Japanese language|Japanese]] in this way. However, such influence does not constitute (and is not a measure of) a genetic relationship between the languages concerned. Linguistic interference can occur between languages that are genetically closely related, between languages that are distantly related (like English and French, which are distantly related [[Indo-European language]]s) and between languages that have no genetic relationship.
: '''''See main article, [[Native American languages]]'''''
# [[Alacalufan languages]] ([[South America]]) (2)
# [[Algic languages]] ([[North American]] & [[Central America]]) (29)
# [[Arauan languages]] ([[South America]]) (8)
# [[Araucanian languages]] ([[South America]]) (2)
# [[Arawakan languages]] ([[South America]], [[Caribbean]]) (60)
# [[Arutani-Sape languages]] ([[South America]]) (2)
# [[Aymaran languages]] ([[South America]]) (3)
# [[Barbacoan languages]] ([[South America]]) (7)
# [[Cahuapanan languages]] ([[South America]]) (2)
# [[Carib languages]] ([[South America]]) (29)
# [[Chapacura-Wanham languages]] ([[South America]]) (5)
# [[Chibchan languages]] ([[Central America]] & [[South America]]) (22)
# [[Choco languages]] ([[South America]]) (10)
# [[Chon languages]] ([[South America]]) (2)
# [[Comecrudan languages]] ([[North America]] & [[Central America]]) (3)
# [[Guacurian languages]] (a.k.a. Waikurian) (8)
# [[Harakmbet languages]] ([[South America]]) (2)
# [[Jicaquean languages]] ([[Central America]])
# [[Jivaroan languages]] ([[South America]]) (4)
# [[Katukinan languages]] ([[South America]]) (3)
# [[Lencan languages]] ([[Central America]])
# [[Lule-Vilela languages]] ([[South America]]) (1)
# [[Macro-Ge languages]] ([[South America]]) (32)
# [[Maku languages]] ([[South America]]) (6)
# [[Mascoian languages]] ([[South America]]) (5)
# [[Mataco-Guaicuru languages]] ([[South America]]) (11)
# [[Mayan languages]] ([[Central America]]) (31)
# [[Misumalpan languages]] ([[Central America]])
# [[Mixe-Zoquean languages]] ([[Central America]]) (19)
# [[Mosetenan languages]] ([[South America]]) (1)
# [[Mura languages]] ([[South America]]) (1)
# [[Na-Dene languages|Na-Den&eacute; languages]] ([[North America]] & [[Central America]]) (40)
# [[Nambiquaran languages]] ([[South America]]) (5)
# [[Oto-Manguean languages]] ([[North America]] & [[Central America]]) (27)
# [[Paezan languages]] ([[South America]]) (1)
# [[Panoan languages]] ([[South America]]) (30)
# [[Peba-Yaguan languages]] ([[South America]]) (2)
# [[Quechuan languages]] ([[South America]]) (46)
# [[Salivan languages]] ([[South America]]) (2)
# [[Tacanan languages]] ([[South America]]) (6)
# [[Tequistlatecan languages]] ([[Central America]]) (3)
# [[Totonacan languages]] ([[Central America]]) (2)
# [[Tucanoan languages]] ([[South America]]) (25)
# [[Tupi languages]] ([[South America]]) (70)
# [[Uru-Chipaya languages]] ([[South America]]) (2)
# [[Uto-Aztecan languages]] ([[North America]] & [[Central America]]) (31)
# [[Witotoan languages]] ([[South America]]) (6)
# [[Xincan languages]] ([[Central America]])
# [[Yanomam languages]] ([[South America]]) (4)
# [[Yuman-Cochimi languages]] ([[North America]] & [[Central America]]) (11)
# [[Zamucoan languages]] ([[South America]]) (2)
# [[Zaparoan languages]] ([[South America]]) (7)
 
===Complications===
== [[Language isolates]] (spoken) ==
Some exceptions to the simple genetic relationship model of languages include [[language isolate]]s and [[Mixed language|mixed]], [[pidgin]] and [[creole language]]s.
 
Mixed languages, pidgins and creole languages constitute special genetic types of languages. They do not descend linearly or directly from a single language and have no single ancestor.
===Central & South America===
# Aikaná ''(Brazil: Rondônia)
# Alagüilac ''(Guatemala)''
# [[Andoque language|Andoque]] ''(Colombia, Peru)''
# Baenan ''(Brazil)''
# Betoi ''(Columbia)''
# [[Camsá language|Camsá]] ''(Columbia)''
# Canichana ''(Bolivia)''
# [[Cayubaba language]] ''(Bolivia)''
# Coahuilteco ''(US: Texas; northeast Mexico)''
# Cofán ''(Colombia, Ecuador)''
# [[Cotoname language|Cotoname]] ''(northeast Mexico; US: Texas)''
# Cuitlatec ''(Mexico: Guerrero)''
# Culle ''(Peru)''
# Cunza ''(Chile, Bolivia, Argentina)''
# Gamela ''(Brazil: Maranhão)''
# Gorgotoqui ''(Bolivia)''
# Huamoé ''(Brazil: Pernambuco)''
# Huave ''(Mexico: Oaxaca)''
# Irantxe ''(Brazil: Mato Grosso)''
# [[Itonama language|Itonama]] ''(Bolivia)''
# Jotí ''(Venezuela)''
# Karirí ''(Brazil: Paraíba, Pernambuco, Ceará)
# Koayá ''(Brazil: Rondônia)
# Kukurá ''(Brazil: Mato Grosso)''
# Mapudungu ''(Chile, Argentina)''
# Maratino ''(northeastern Mexico)''
# Movima ''(Bolivia)''
# Munichi ''(Peru)''
# Nambiquaran ''(Brazil: Mato Grosso)''
# Naolan ''(Mexico: Tamaulipas)''
# Natú ''(Brazil: Pernambuco)''
# Omurano ''(Peru)''
# Otí ''(Brazil: São Paulo)''
# [[Pankararú language|Pankararú]] ''(Brazil: Pernambuco)''
# [[Puelche language|Puelche]] ''(Chile)''
# [[Puinave language|Puinave]] ''(Columbia)''
# Puquina ''(Bolivia)''
# Quinigua ''(northeast Mexico)''
# Sabela ''(Ecuador, Peru)''
# Seri ''(Mexico: Sonora)''
# Solano ''(northeast Mexico; US: Texas)''
# Tarairiú ''(Brazil: Rio Grande do Norte)''
# [[Tarascan]] (a.k.a. Purépecha) ''(Mexico: Michoacán)''
# Taushiro ''(Peru)''
# Tequiraca ''(Peru)''
# [[Ticuna language|Ticuna]] ''(Colombia, Peru, Brazil)''
# [[Tuxá language|Tuxá]] ''(Brazil: Bahia, Pernambuco)''
# [[Warao language|Warao]] ''(Guyana, Surinam, Venezuela)''
# Xokó ''(Brazil: Alagoas, Pernambuco)''
# Xukurú ''(Brazil: Pernambuco, Paraíba)''
# [[Yámana language|Yámana]] (a.k.a Yagan) ''(Chile)''
# [[Yuracare language|Yuracare]] ''(Bolivia)''
# Yuri ''(Colombia, Brazil)''
# Yurumanguí ''(Columbia)''
 
[[Language isolate|Isolates]] are languages that cannot be proven to be genealogically related to any other modern language. As a corollary, every language isolate also forms its own language family — a genetic family which happens to consist of just one language. One often cited example is [[Basque language|Basque]], which forms a language family on its own; but there are many other examples outside Europe. On the global scale, the site [[Glottolog]] counts a total of 423 language families in the world, including 184 isolates.<ref>Cf. [https://glottolog.org/glottolog/family Language families], [[Glottolog]].</ref>
===North America===
# [[Adai]] ''(US: Louisiana, Texas)''
# Aranama-Tamique ''(US: Texas)''
# {{ll|Atakapa}} ''(US: Louisiana, Texas)''
# [[Beothuk language|Beothuk]] ''(Canada: Newfoundland)''
# [[Calusa]] ''(US: Florida)''
# [[Cayuse]] ''(US: Oregon, Washington)''
# [[Chimariko]] ''(US: California)''
# [[Chitimacha]] ''(US: Lousiania)''
# [[Coahuilteco]] ''(US: Texas; northeast Mexico)''
# [[Cotoname]] ''(northeast Mexico; US: Texas)''
# [[Esselen]] ''(US: California)''
# [[Haida]] ''(Canada: British Columbia; US: Alaska)''
# [[Karankawa]] ''(US: Texas)''
# [[Karok]] (a.k.a. Karuk) ''(US: California)''
# [[Keres language|Keres]] ''(US: New Mexico)''
# [[Konomihu]] ''(US: California)''
# [[Kootenai (tribe)|Kootenai]] ''(Canada: British Columbia; US: Idaho, Montana)''
# [[Natchez (people)|Natchez]] ''(US: Mississippi, Louisiana)''
# [[Salinan]] ''(US: California)''
# [[Siuslaw (tribe)|Siuslaw]] ''(US: Oregon)''
# [[Solano]] ''(northeast Mexico; US: Texas)''
# [[Takelma]] ''(US: Oregon)''
# [[Timucua]] ''(US: Florida, Georgia)''
# [[Tonkawa]] ''(US: Texas)''
# [[Tunica (language)|Tunica]] ''(US: Mississippi, Louisiana, Arkansas)''
# [[Washo language|Washo]] ''(US: California, Nevada)''
# [[Yana]] ''(US: California)''
# [[Yuchi language|Yuchi]] ''(US: Georgia, Oklahoma)''
# [[Zuni language|Zuni]] (a.k.a. Shiwi) ''(US: New Mexico)''
 
===AsiaMonogenesis===
One controversial theory concerning the genetic relationships among languages is [[Proto-Human language|monogenesis]], the idea that all known languages, with the exceptions of [[creole language|creoles]], pidgins and [[sign language]]s, are descendant from a single ancestral language.<ref>[[Johanna Nichols|Nichols, Johanna]]. Monogenesis or Polygenesis: A Single Ancestral Language for All Humanity? Ch. 58 of ''The Oxford Handbook of Language Evolution'', ed. by [[Maggie Tallerman]] and Kathleen Rita Gibson. Oxford: Oxford UP, 2012. 558–72. Print.</ref> If that is true, it would mean all languages (other than pidgins, creoles, and sign languages) are genetically related, but in many cases, the relationships may be too remote to be detectable. Alternative explanations for some basic observed commonalities between languages include developmental theories, related to the biological development of the capacity for language as the child grows from newborn.{{citation needed|date=January 2022}}
# [[Ainu language]] or languages ''(Russia, Japan)'' (like [[Arabic language|Arabic]] or [[Japanese language|Japanese]], the diversity within Ainu is large enough that some consider it to be perhaps up to a dozen languages while others consider it a single language with high dialectal diversity)
# [[Burushaski language|Burushaski]] ''(Pakistan, India)'' (sometimes linked to [[Yeniseian languages|Yeniseian]])
# [[Kalto]] or Nahali ''(India)'' [sometimes linked to Munda]
# [[Korean language|Korean]] ''(North & South Korea, China, USA)'' (sometimes linked to [[Altaic languages|Altaic]])
# [[Nivkh language|Nivkh]] or Gilyak ''(Russia)'' (sometimes linked to [[Chukotko-Kamchatkan languages|Chukchi-Kamchatkan]])
# [[Sumerian language|Sumerian]] ''(Iraq)'' [extinct]
# [[Elamite]] ''(Iran)'' [extinct] (sometimes linked to [[Dravidian language|Dravidian]])
# [[Hattic]] ''(Turkey)'' [extinct] (sometimes linked to [[Northwest_Caucasian_languages|Northwest Caucasian]])
 
== Structure of a family ==
===Africa===
{{More citations needed section|date=May 2022}}
# [[Hadza language|Hadza]] ''(Tanzania)'' (sometimes included in Khoisan)
A language family is a [[monophyly|monophyletic]] unit; all its members derive from a common ancestor, and all descendants of that ancestor are included in the family. Thus, the term ''family'' is analogous to the biological term ''[[clade]]''. Language families can be divided into smaller phylogenetic units, sometimes referred to as "branches" or "subfamilies" of the family; for instance, the [[Germanic languages]] are a subfamily of the [[Indo-European languages|Indo-European]] family. Subfamilies share a more recent common ancestor than the common ancestor of the larger family; [[Proto-Germanic]], the common ancestor of the Germanic subfamily, was itself a descendant of [[Proto-Indo-European]], the common ancestor of the Indo-European family. Within a large family, subfamilies can be identified through "shared innovations": members of a subfamily will share features that represent retentions from their more recent common ancestor, but were not present in the overall proto-language of the larger family.
 
Some [[taxonomy (general)|taxonomists]] restrict the term ''family'' to a certain level, but there is little consensus on how to do so. Those who affix such labels also subdivide branches into ''groups'', and groups into ''complexes''. A top-level (i.e., the largest) family is often called a ''phylum'' or ''stock''. The closer the branches are to each other, the more closely the languages will be related. This means if a branch of a [[proto-language]] is four branches down and there is also a [[sister language]] to that fourth branch, then the two sister languages are more closely related to each other than to that common ancestral proto-language.
===Europe===
# [[Basque language|Basque]] ''(Spain, France)''
# [[Etruscan language|Etruscan]] ''(Italy)'' [extinct]
# [[Pictish_language#Pictish_language|Pictish]] ''(Scotland)'' [extinct] [disputed - possibly an Indo European [[Brythonic_languages|Celtic - Brythonic]] language]
 
The term ''[[macrofamily]]'' or ''superfamily'' is sometimes applied to proposed groupings of language families whose status as phylogenetic units is generally considered to be unsubstantiated by accepted [[historical linguistics|historical linguistic]] methods.
== [[Sign language]]s ==
:''See [[List of sign languages]]''
* [[Adamorobe Sign Language]] (ADS)
* [[American Sign Language]] (ASL)
* [[Auslan|Australian Sign Language]] (Auslan)
* [[Austrian Sign Language]] "Österreichische Gebärdensprache" (ÖGS)
* [[BANZSL]] - 'Parent' language of which [[British Sign Language|BSL]], [[Auslan]], and [[NZSL]] can be considered dialects
* [[Brazilian Sign Language]] "Língua Brasileira de Sinais" (LIBRAS)
* [[British Sign Language]] (BSL)
* [[Chinese Sign Language]] "&#20013;&#22269;&#25163;&#35821;" (CSL)
* [[Danish Sign Language]]
* [[Dutch Sign Language]] "Nederlandse Gebarentaal" (NGT)
* [[Finnish Sign Language]] "Suomalainen viittomakieli" (SVK)
* [[Flemish Sign Language]] "Vlaamse Gebarentaal" (VGT)
* [[French Sign Language]] "Langue des Signes Française" (LSF)
* [[German Sign Language]] "Deutsche Gebärdensprache" (DGS)
* [[Hawaii Pidgin Sign Language]]
* [[Indian Sign Language]]
* [[Italian Sign Language]] "Lingua dei Segni Italiana"
* [[Irish Sign Language]] (ISL)
* [[Japanese Sign Language]] "&#26085;&#26412;&#25163;&#35441;" (''Nihon shuwa''), (JSL)
* [[Malaysian Sign Language]] "Bahasa Isyarat Malaysia" (BIM)
* [[Maltese Sign Language]] "Lingwi tas-Sinjali Maltin" (LSM)
* [[Martha's Vineyard Sign Language]]
* [[Mexican Sign Language]] "Lengua de Señas Mexicana"
* [[New Zealand Sign Language]] (NZSL)
* [[Nicaraguan Sign Language]] "Idioma de Signos Nicaragüense" (ISN)
* [[Penang Sign Language]] (used in Malaysia)
* [[Polish Sign Language]] "Polski J&#281;zyk Migowy" (PJM)
* [[Portuguese Sign Language]] "Lingua Gestual Portuguesa" (LGP)
* [[Quebec Sign Language]] "Langue des Signes Québécois" (LSQ)
* [[Selangor Sign Language]] (used in Malaysia)
* [[South African Sign Language]] (SASL)
* [[Spanish Sign Language]] "Lenguaje de Signos Español" (LSE)
* [[Swedish Sign Language]] "tecknad svenska" (TS)
* [[Swiss-French Sign Language]] "Langage Gestuelle"
* [[Swiss-German Sign Language]] "Deutschschweizer Gebärdensprache" (DSGS)
* [[Taiwanese Sign Language]] (TSL)
* [[Yucatec Maya Sign Language]]
 
=== Dialect continua ===
== [[Creole language]]s, [[pidgin]]s, [[mixed language]]s, and [[trade languages]] ==
{{main|Dialect continuum}}
* [[American Indian Pidgin English]]
* [[Basque-Algonquian Pidgin]] (a.k.a. Micmac-Basque Pidgin, Souriquois)
* [[Bislamic languages]]
** [[Bislama]]
** [[Broken language|Broken]]
** [[Pijin]]
** [[Tok Pisin]]
* [[Broken Oghibbeway]] (a.k.a. Broken Ojibwa)
* [[Broken Slavey]] (a.k.a. Slavey Jargon, Broken Slavé)
* [[Callahuaya]] (a.k.a. Machaj-Juyai, Kallawaya)
* [[Carib Pidgin]] (a.k.a. Ndjuka-Amerindian Pidgin, Ndjuka-Trio)
* [[Carib Pidgin-Arawak Mixed Language]]
* [[Catalangu]]
* [[Chabacano]] - A Spanish creole spoken in South of the Philippines.
* [[Chinook Jargon]]
* [[Delaware Jargon]] (a.k.a. Pidgin Delaware)
* [[Eskimo Trade Jargon]] (a.k.a. Herschel Island Eskimo Pidgin, Ship's Jargon)
* [[Greenlandic Eskimo Pidgin]]
* [[Guajiro-Spanish]]
* [[Güegüence-Nicarao]]
* [[Haida Jargon]]
* [[Haitian creole]]
* [[Hawaiian Creole English]]
* [[Hiri Motu]]
* [[Hudson Strait Pidgin]]
* [[International Sign]] or Gestuno - [[constructed language]]
* [[Inuktitut-English Pidgin]]
* [[Jargonized Powhatan]]
* [[Kutenai Jargon]]
* [[Labrador Eskimo Pidgin]] (a.k.a. Labrador Inuit Pidgin)
* [[Lingua Franca Apalachee]]
* [[Lingua Franca Creek]]
* [[Lingua franca]]
* [[Lingua Geral do Sul]] (a.k.a. Lingua Geral Paulista, Tupí Austral)
* [[Loucheux Jargon]] (a.k.a. Jargon Loucheux)
* [[Media Lengua]]
* [[Mednyj Aleut]] (a.k.a. Copper Island Aleut, Medniy Aleut, CIA)
* [[Michif]] (a.k.a. French Cree, Métis, Metchif, Mitchif, Métchif)
* [[Mobilian Jargon]] (a.k.a. Mobilian Trade Jargon, Chickasaw-Chocaw Trade Language, Yamá
* [[Montagnais Pidgin Basque]] (a.k.a. Pidgin Basque-Montagnais)
* [[Nheengatú]] (a.k.a. Lingua Geral Amazônica, Lingua Boa, Lingua Brasílica, Lingua Geral do Norte)
* [[Norfuk_language | Norfuk]]
* [[Nootka Jargon]]
* [[Ocaneechi]]
* [[Pitcairnese_language | Pitkern]]
* [[Pidgin Massachusett]]
* [[Portuguese Creole]] languages
* [[Rusnorsk]]
* [[Sango (language)|Sango]]
 
Some close-knit language families, and many branches within larger families, take the form of [[dialect continuum|dialect continua]] in which there are no clear-cut geographical boundaries that make it possible to unequivocally identify, define, or count individual languages within the family. However, when the differences between the speech of different regions at the extremes of the continuum are so great that there is no [[mutual intelligibility]] between them, as occurs in [[Arabic]], the continuum cannot meaningfully be seen as a single language.
== Proposed language stocks ==
{|
| valign="top" |
* [[Alarodian languages|Alarodian]]
* [[Almosan]] (= Sapir's ''Algonkin-Wakashan'')
* [[Almosan-Keresiouan]]
* [[Algonkian-Gulf]]
* [[Amerind languages|Amerind]]
** [[Central Amerind]]
* [[Andean languages]]
* [[Aztec-Tanoan]]
* [[Austric languages|Austric]]
* [[Chibchan-Paezan]]
* [[Coahuiltecan]]
* [[Dene-Caucasian languages|Dene-Caucasian]]
* [[Equatorial languages]]
* [[Eurasiatic languages|Eurasiatic]]
* [[Gulf languages]]
* [[Hokan languages]]
* [[Hokan-Siouan]]
* [[Ibero-Caucasian languages|Ibero-Caucasian]]
* [[Indo-Pacific languages|Indo-Pacific]]
| valign="top" |
* [[Keresiouan]]
* [[Kongo-Saharan languages|Kongo-Saharan]]
* [[Macro-Carib]]
* [[Macro-Ge]]
* [[Macro-Mayan]]
* [[Macro-Panoan]]
* [[Macro-Siouan languages|Macro-Siouan]]
* [[Macro-Tucanoan]]
* [[Mosan]]
* [[Na-Dene]] (Sapir's)
* [[Nostratic languages|Nostratic]]
* [[Nostratic-Amerind]]
* [[Penutian languages|Penutian]]
* [[Proto-Pontic|Pontic]]
* [[Proto-World language|Proto-World]]
* [[Quechumaran]]
* [[Ural-Altaic languages|Ural-Altaic]]
* [[Wappo-Yukian]]
|}
 
Further, a speech variety may either be considered as a language or as a dialect, depending on social or political considerations. Thus, different sources, especially over time, can give wildly different numbers of languages within a certain family. [[Classification of the Japonic languages|Classifications of the Japonic family]], for example, range from one language (a language isolate with dialects) to nearly twenty—until the classification of [[Ryukyuan language|Ryukyuan]] as separate languages within a [[Japonic language family]] rather than dialects of Japanese, the [[Japanese language]] itself was considered a [[language isolate]] and therefore the only language in its family.
== Other natural languages of special interest ==
* [[Endangered languages]]
* [[Extinct languages]]
* [[Constructed languages]]
 
=== External linksIsolates ===
{{main|Language isolate}}
*http://www.ethnologue.com/web.asp
Most of the world's languages are known to be related to others. Those that have no known relatives (or for which family relationships are only tentatively proposed) are called [[language isolate]]s, essentially language families consisting of a single language. There are an estimated 129 language isolates known today.<ref name=":0">{{cite journal|last=Campbell |first=Lyle |date=24 August 2010 |title=Language Isolates and Their History, or, What's Weird, Anyway? |journal=Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society |language=en |volume=36 |issue=1 |pages=16–31 |doi=10.3765/bls.v36i1.3900 |issn=2377-1666 |doi-access=free}}</ref> An example is [[Basque language|Basque]]. In general, it is assumed that language isolates have relatives or had relatives at some point in their history but at a time depth too great for linguistic comparison to recover them.
<!--*http://www.unilang2.org/main/families.php broken link -->
 
*http://gebaren.ugent.be
A language isolate is classified based on the fact that enough is known about the isolate to compare it genetically to other languages but no common ancestry or relationship is found with any other known language.<ref name=":0" />
*http://www.elanguages.info - articles, products, & info about language learning online
 
*[http://www.nicemice.net/amc/tmp/lang-pop.var Number of speakers by language]
A language isolated in its own branch within a family, such as [[Albanian language|Albanian]] and [[Armenian language|Armenian]] within Indo-European, is often also called an isolate, but the meaning of the word "isolate" in such cases is usually clarified with a [[Grammatical modifier|modifier]]. For instance, Albanian and Armenian may be referred to as an "Indo-European isolate". By contrast, so far as is known, the [[Basque language]] is an absolute isolate: it has not been shown to be related to any other modern language despite numerous attempts. A language may be said to be an isolate currently but not historically if related but now extinct relatives are attested. The [[Aquitanian language]], spoken in Roman times, may have been an ancestor of Basque, but it could also have been a sister language to the ancestor of Basque. In the latter case, Basque and Aquitanian would form a small family together. Ancestors are not considered to be distinct members of a family.{{citation needed|date=April 2023}}
 
=== Proto-languages ===
{{main|Proto-language}}
A proto-language can be thought of as a mother language (not to be confused with a [[mother tongue]]<ref>{{cite book|last=Bloomfield |first=Leonard |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=Gfrd-On5iFwC |title=Language |year=1994 |publisher=Motilal Banarsidass Publ. |isbn=81-208-1196-8}}</ref>) being the root from which all languages in the family stem. The common ancestor of a language family is seldom known directly since most languages have a relatively short recorded history. However, it is possible to recover many features of a proto-language by applying the [[comparative method]], a reconstructive procedure worked out by 19th century linguist [[August Schleicher]]. This can demonstrate the validity of many of the proposed families in the [[list of language families]]. For example, the reconstructible common ancestor of the Indo-European language family is called ''[[Proto-Indo-European language|Proto-Indo-European]]''. Proto-Indo-European is not attested by written records and so is conjectured to have been spoken before the invention of writing.
 
==Visual representation==
[[File:20150714040758!Mayan languages tree gl.svg|thumb|upright=1.35|An example of a language tree, containing the Mayan languages]]
A common visual representation of a language family is given by a genetic language tree. The [[tree model]] is sometimes termed a [[dendrogram]] or [[phylogeny]]. The family tree shows the relationship of the languages within a family, much as a family tree of an individual shows their relationship with their relatives. There are criticisms to the family tree model. Critics focus mainly on the claim that the internal structure of the trees is subject to variation based on the criteria of classification.<ref>Edzard, Lutz. ''[https://books.google.com/books?id=UAFVgdGljl4C Polygenesis, Convergence, and Entropy: An Alternative Model of Linguistic Evolution Applied to Semitic Linguistics]''. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz, 1998. Print.</ref> Even among those who support the family tree model, there are debates over which languages should be included in a language family. For example, within the dubious [[Altaic language family]], there are debates over whether the [[Japonic]] and [[Koreanic]] languages should be included or not.<ref>Georg, Stefan, Peter A. Michalove, Alexis Manaster Ramer, and Paul J. Sidwell. Telling General Linguists about Altaic. ''Journal of Linguistics'' 35.1 (1999): 65–98. Print.</ref>
 
The [[wave model]] has been proposed as an alternative to the tree model.<ref name="francois">{{cite book |last=François |first=Alexandre |author-link=Alexandre François |contribution= Trees, Waves and Linkages: Models of Language Diversification |editor1-last=Bowern |editor1-first=Claire |editor2-last=Evans |editor2-first=Bethwyn |title=The Routledge Handbook of Historical Linguistics |pages=161–189 |publisher=Routledge |place=London |year=2014 |isbn=978-0-41552-789-7 |contribution-url=http://alex.francois.online.fr/data/AlexFrancois_2014_HHL_Trees-waves-linkages_Diversification.pdf}}</ref> The wave model uses [[isogloss]]es to group language varieties; unlike in the tree model, these groups can overlap. While the tree model implies a lack of contact between languages after derivation from an ancestral form, the wave model emphasizes the relationship between languages that remain in contact, which is more realistic.<ref name="francois"/> [[Historical glottometry]] is an application of the wave model, meant to identify and evaluate genetic relations in [[Linkage (linguistics)|linguistic linkages]].<ref name="francois"/><ref>{{cite book |last1=Kalyan |first1=Siva |author1-link= |last2=François |first2=Alexandre |author2-link= |contribution=Freeing the Comparative Method from the tree model: A framework for Historical Glottometry |editor1-last=Kikusawa |editor1-first=Ritsuko |editor2-last=Reid |editor2-first=Laurie |title=Let's Talk about Trees: Genetic Relationships of Languages and Their Phylogenic Representation |series=Senri Ethnological Studies |volume=98 |pages=59–89 |publisher=National Museum of Ethnology |place=Ōsaka |year=2018 |isbn= |contribution-url=http://alex.francois.online.fr/data/Kalyan-Francois_2018_Freeing-Comparative-Method-from-Tree-model.pdf}}</ref>
 
== Other classifications of languages ==
{{More citations needed section|date=May 2022}}
 
=== Sprachbund ===
{{Main|Sprachbund}}
 
A [[sprachbund]] is a geographic area having several languages that feature common linguistic structures. The similarities between those languages are caused by language contact, not by chance or common origin, and are not recognized as criteria that define a language family. An example of a sprachbund would be the [[Indian subcontinent]].<ref>{{cite web|last=Joseph |first=Brian |date=2017 |title=The Balkan Sprachbund |url=https://www.linguisticsociety.org/sites/default/files/e-learning/LSAInstitute--BalkanSprachbundSlides.pdf|access-date=2 October 2020 |website=linguisticsociety.org}}</ref>
 
Shared innovations, acquired by borrowing or other means, are not considered genetic and have no bearing with the language family concept. It has been asserted, for example, that many of the more striking features shared by [[Italic languages]] ([[Latin]], [[Oscan language|Oscan]], [[Umbrian language|Umbrian]], etc.) might well be "[[areal feature]]s". However, very similar-looking alterations in the systems of long vowels in the [[West Germanic languages]] greatly postdate any possible notion of a proto-language innovation (and cannot readily be regarded as "areal", either, since English and continental West Germanic were not a linguistic area). In a similar vein, there are many similar unique innovations in [[Germanic languages|Germanic]], [[Baltic languages|Baltic]] and [[Slavic languages|Slavic]] that are far more likely to be areal features than traceable to a common proto-language. But legitimate uncertainty about whether shared innovations are areal features, coincidence, or inheritance from a common ancestor, leads to disagreement over the proper subdivisions of any large language family.
 
=== Contact languages ===
{{main|Mixed language|Creole language|Pidgin}}
The concept of language families is based on the historical observation that languages develop [[dialect]]s, which over time may diverge into distinct languages. However, linguistic ancestry is less clear-cut than familiar biological ancestry, in which species do not crossbreed.<ref name="ListNelson-Sathi2014">{{cite journal|last1=List |first1=Johann-Mattis |last2=Nelson-Sathi |first2=Shijulal |last3=Geisler |first3=Hans |last4=Martin |first4=William |title=Networks of lexical borrowing and lateral gene transfer in language and genome evolution |journal=[[BioEssays]] |volume=36 |issue=2 |year=2014 |pages=141–150 |issn=0265-9247 |doi=10.1002/bies.201300096 |pmid=24375688 |pmc=3910147}}</ref> It is more like the evolution of microbes, with extensive [[lateral gene transfer]]. Quite distantly related languages may affect each other through [[language contact]], which in extreme cases may lead to languages with no single ancestor, whether they be [[creole language|creoles]] or [[mixed language]]s. In addition, a number of [[sign language]]s have developed in isolation and appear to have no relatives at all. Nonetheless, such cases are relatively rare and most well-attested languages can be unambiguously classified as belonging to one language family or another, even if this family's relation to other families is not known.
 
Language contact can lead to the development of new languages from the mixture of two or more languages for the purposes of interactions between two groups who speak different languages. Languages that arise in order for two groups to communicate with each other to engage in commercial trade or that appeared as a result of colonialism are called [[pidgins|pidgin]]. Pidgins are an example of linguistic and cultural expansion caused by language contact. However, language contact can also lead to cultural divisions. In some cases, two different language speaking groups can feel territorial towards their language and do not want any changes to be made to it. This causes language boundaries and groups in contact are not willing to make any compromises to accommodate the other language.<ref>{{cite web|title=Languages in Contact {{!}} Linguistic Society of America |url=https://www.linguisticsociety.org/resource/languages-contact |access-date=2 October 2020 |website=www.linguisticsociety.org}}</ref>
 
== See also ==
{{Colbegin}}
* [[Comparative linguistics]]
* [[Constructed language]]
* [[Endangered language]]
* [[Extinct language]]
* [[Language death]]
* [[Language isolate]]
* [[List of revived languages]]
* [[Global language system]]
* [[ISO 639-5]]
* [[Linguist List]]
* [[List of language families]]
* [[List of languages by number of native speakers]]
* [[Origin of language]]
* [[Proto-language]]
* [[Proto-Human language]]
* [[Sprachbund]]
* [[Tree model]]
* [[Unclassified language]]
* [[Father Tongue hypothesis]]
* [[Farming/language dispersal hypothesis]]
 
{{Colend}}
{{clear}}
 
==Notes==
{{reflist|group=note}}
 
== Bibliography References==
{{Reflist}}
 
== Further reading ==
* Boas, Franz. (1911). ''Handbook of American Indian languages'' (Vol. 1). Bureau of American Ethnology, Bulletin 40. Washington: Government Print Office (Smithsonian Institution, Bureau of American Ethnology).
{{refbegin}}
* Boas, Franz. (1922). ''Handbook of American Indian languages'' (Vol. 2). Bureau of American Ethnology, Bulletin 40. Washington: Government Print Office (Smithsonian Institution, Bureau of American Ethnology).
* {{cite book|last=Boas |first=Franz |year=1911 |title=Handbook of American Indian languages |volume=1 |series=Bureau of American Ethnology, Bulletin 40 |___location=Washington |publisher=[[Smithsonian Institution]], Bureau of American Ethnology |isbn=0-8032-5017-7}}
* Boas, Franz. (1922). ''Handbook of American Indian languages'' (Vol. 2). Bureau of American Ethnology, Bulletin 40. Washington, D.C.: Government Print Office (Smithsonian Institution, Bureau of American Ethnology).
* Boas, Franz. (1933). ''Handbook of American Indian languages'' (Vol. 3). Native American legal materials collection, title 1227. Glückstadt: J.J. Augustin.
* Campbell, Lyle. (1997). ''American Indian languages: The historical linguistics of Native America''. New York: Oxford University Press. {{ISBN |0-19-509427-1}}.
* Campbell, Lyle; & Mithun, Marianne (Eds.). (1979). ''The languages of native America: Historical and comparative assessment''. Austin: University of Texas Press.
* Goddard, Ives (Ed.). (1996). ''Languages''. Handbook of North American Indians (W. C. Sturtevant, General Ed.) (Vol. 17). Washington, D. C.: Smithsonian Institution. {{ISBN |0-160416-8774048774-9}}.
* Goddard, Ives. (1999). ''Native languages and language families of North America'' (rev. and enlarged ed. with additions and corrections). [Map]. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press (Smithsonian InstituteInstitution). (Updated version of the map in Goddard 1996). {{ISBN |0-8032-9271-6}}.
* Gordon, Raymond G., Jr. (Ed.). (2005). ''Ethnologue: Languages of the world'' (15th ed.). Dallas, TX: SIL International. {{ISBN |1-55671-159-X}}. (Online version: [http://www.ethnologue.com Ethnologue: Languages of the World]).
* Greenberg, Joseph H. (1966). ''The Languages of Africa'' (2nd ed.). Bloomington: Indiana University.
* Harrison, K. David. (2007) ''When Languages Die: The Extinction of the World's Languages and the Erosion of Human Knowledge''. New York and London: Oxford University Press.
* Mithun, Marianne. (1999). ''The languages of Native North America''. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-23228-7 (hbk); ISBN 0-521-29875-X.
* Mithun, Marianne. (1999). ''The languages of Native North America''. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. {{ISBN|0-521-23228-7}} (hbk); {{ISBN|0-521-29875-X}}.
* Ross, Malcolm. (2005). "[http://rspas.anu.edu.au/linguistics/mdr/Papuan%20Pasts%20paper.pdf Pronouns as a preliminary diagnostic for grouping Papuan languages] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20040608222723/http://rspas.anu.edu.au/linguistics/mdr/Papuan%20Pasts%20paper.pdf |date=8 June 2004 }}". In: [[Andrew Pawley]], Robert Attenborough, Robin Hide and Jack Golson, eds, ''Papuan pasts: cultural, linguistic and biological histories of Papuan-speaking peoples'' (PDF)
* Ruhlen, Merritt. (1987). ''A guide to the world's languages''. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
* Sturtevant, William C. (Ed.). (1978-present1978–present). ''Handbook of North American Indians'' (Vol. 1-201–20). Washington, D. C.: Smithsonian Institution. (Vols. 1-31–3, 16, 18-2018–20 not yet published).
* Voegelin, C. F.; & Voegelin, F. M. (1977). ''Classification and index of the world's languages''. New York: Elsevier.
{{refend}}
 
== External links ==
* [http://www.muturzikin.com/countries.htm Linguistic maps] (from Muturzikin)
* [https://www.ethnologue.com/ Ethnologue]
* [http://multitree.org/ The Multitree Project]
* [http://www.proel.org/index.php?pagina=mundo Lenguas del mundo] (World Languages)
* [http://en.wiktionary.org/wiki/Appendix:Swadesh_lists Comparative Swadesh list tables of various language families] (from Wiktionary)
* [http://www.ezglot.com/most-similar-languages.php Most similar languages]
 
[[Category:{{Language families|*]]}}
[[Category:Lists of languages|*]]
 
{{DEFAULTSORT:Language Family}}
[[ar:&#1593;&#1575;&#1574;&#1604;&#1575;&#1578; &#1604;&#1594;&#1608;&#1610;&#1577;]]
[[Category:Historical linguistics]]
[[af:Taalfamilies]]
[[Category:Language families| ]]
[[an:Familias_lingüisticas]]
[[Category:Types of groupings]]
[[ast:Familia (llingüistica)]]
[[bn:&#2477;&#2494;&#2487;&#2494; &#2474;&#2480;&#2495;&#2476;&#2494;&#2480;&#2488;&#2478;&#2498;&#2489;]]
[[cs:Seznam jazyk&#367; (podle skupin)]]
[[de:Sprachfamilien]]
[[eo:Lingva familio]]
[[es:Familias de lenguas]]
[[fr:Langues par famille]]
[[ia:Familias linguistic]]
[[id:Rumpun bahasa]]
[[it:Famiglie di linguaggi]]
[[ja:&#35328;&#35486;&#12398;&#20998;&#39006;&#19968;&#35239;]]
[[ko:&#50612;&#51313;]]
[[lt:Kalb&#371; &#353;eimos]]
[[nl:Taalfamilies]]
[[no:Språkfamilier]]
[[pl:Rodzina j&#281;zykowa]]
[[ro:Familii de limbi]]
[[ru:&#1043;&#1077;&#1085;&#1077;&#1072;&#1083;&#1086;&#1075;&#1080;&#1095;&#1077;&#1089;&#1082;&#1086;&#1077; &#1076;&#1077;&#1088;&#1077;&#1074;&#1086; &#1103;&#1079;&#1099;&#1082;&#1086;&#1074;]]
[[simple:Language families and languages]]
[[sl:Jezikovne dru&#382;ine in jeziki]]
[[ta:மொழிகளும், மொழிக் குடும்பங்களும்]]
[[th:&#3605;&#3619;&#3632;&#3585;&#3641;&#3621;&#3586;&#3629;&#3591;&#3616;&#3634;&#3625;&#3634;]]
[[tl:Pamilya ng mga wika]]
[[zh:&#35821;&#35328;&#31995;&#23646;&#20998;&#31867;]]