One-factor-at-a-time method: Difference between revisions

Content deleted Content added
No edit summary
Adding short description: "Method of designing experiments"
 
(13 intermediate revisions by 11 users not shown)
Line 1:
{{Short description|Method of designing experiments}}
The '''one-factor-at-a-time method,'''<ref>{{Cite journal|lastlast1=Razavi|firstfirst1=Saman|last2=Gupta|first2=Hoshin V.|date=2015|title=What do we mean by sensitivity analysis? The need for comprehensive characterization of “global”"global" sensitivity in Earth and Environmental systems models|url=https://agupubs.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1002/2014WR016527|journal=Water Resources Research|language=en|volume=51|issue=5|pages=3070–3092|doi=10.1002/2014wr016527|bibcode=2015WRR....51.3070R |issn=0043-1397|viadoi-access=free}}</ref>, also known as '''one-variable-at-a-time''', '''OFAT''', '''OF@T''',
'''OFaaT''', '''OVAT''', '''OV@T''', '''OVaaT''', or '''monothetic analysis''' is a method of [[design of experiments|designing experiments]] involving the testing of factors, or causes, one at a time instead of multiple factors simultaneously.
 
Line 7 ⟶ 8:
 
There exist cases where the mental effort required to conduct a complex multi-factor analysis exceeds the effort required to acquire extra data, in which case OFAT might make sense. Furthermore, some researchers have shown that OFAT can be more effective than [[Fractional factorial design|fractional factorials]] under certain conditions (number of runs is limited, primary goal is to attain improvements in the system, and experimental error is not large compared to factor effects, which must be additive and independent of each other).<ref name=" Friedman, M., and Savage, L. J. (1947), “Planning Experiments Seeking Maxima,” in Techniques of Statistical Analysis, eds. C. Eisenhart, M. W. Hastay, and W. A. Wallis, New York: McGraw-Hill, pp. 365-372.
">Friedman, M., and Savage, L. J. (1947), “Planning Experiments Seeking Maxima,” in Techniques of Statistical Analysis, eds. C. Eisenhart, M. W. Hastay, and W. A. Wallis, New York: McGraw-Hill, pp. 365-372.</ref><ref name= "Cuthbert Daniel">Daniel , C. (1973) ,“One-at-a-Time Plans,” Journal of the American Statistical Association 68, 353-360</ref>
 
==Disadvantages==
Line 15 ⟶ 16:
Bevington's<ref>Bevington and Robinson, ''Data Reduction and Error Analysis for the Physical Sciences'', 2nd Ed. McGraw&ndash;Hill (1992)</ref> <code>GRIDLS</code> versus <code>GRADLS</code>. The latter is far from optimal, but the former, which changes only one variable at a time, is worse. See also the [[factorial design|factorial experimental design]] methods pioneered by [[Ronald Fisher|Sir Ronald A. Fisher]]. Reasons for disfavoring OFAT include:
 
1. #OFAT requires more runs for the same precision in effect estimation<br />
2. #OFAT cannot estimate interactions<br />
3. #OFAT can miss optimal settings of factors.
 
Designed experiments remain nearly always preferred to OFAT with many types and methods available,<ref>See Category: Experimental design, at bottom.</ref> in addition to fractional factorials which, though usually requiring more runs than OFAT, do address the three concerns above.<ref name=Czitrom>[https://www.questia.com/googleScholar.qst?docId{{cite journal |last=Czitrom|first=Veronica|authorlink=5001888588 Veronica Czitrom (|year=1999) "|title=One-Factor-at-a-Time Versus Designed Experiments", |journal=American Statistician, |volume=53, |issue=2 |pages=126–131 |doi=10.]<2307/ref>2685731|jstor= 2685731}}<!--formerly http://www.amstat.org/publications/tas/czitrom.pdf--ref> One modern design over which OFAT has no advantage in number of runs is the [[Plackett-Burman design|Plackett-Burman]] which, by having all factors vary simultaneously (an important quality in experimental designs),<ref name=Czitrom /> gives generally [[Efficiency (statistics)|greater precision in effect estimation]].
 
==See also==