Extended parallel process model: Difference between revisions

Content deleted Content added
No edit summary
No edit summary
 
(43 intermediate revisions by 17 users not shown)
Line 1:
{{short description|How individuals react to fear-inducing messages}}
{{Multiple issues|
The '''extended parallel process model''' ('''EPPM''') is a fear appeal theory developed by communications scholar [[Kim Witte]] that illustrates how individuals react to fear-inducing messages.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Witte K | title = Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model. | journal = Communication Monographs | date = December 1992 | volume = 59 | issue = 4 | pages = 329–349 | doi = 10.1080/03637759209376276 }}</ref> Witte subsequently published an initial test of the model in [[Communication Monographs]].<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Witte K | title = Fear control and danger control: A test of the extended parallel process model (EPPM). | journal = Communication Monographs | date = June 1994 | volume = 61 | issue = 2 | pages = 113–134 | doi = 10.1080/03637759409376328 }}</ref>
{{refimprove|date=December 2008}}
{{expert-subject|Psychology|date=January 2015}}
}}
 
The EPPM was developed by Witte as a response to the significant inconsistencies in fear appeal literature, serving as an extension of previous fear appeal models, hence the use of 'extended' in name 'EPPM'. The model is originally based on Leventhal's Parallel Process Model – a danger and fear control framework that studied how adaptive protective behaviour stemmed from attempts of danger control.<ref name=":1">{{cite journal | vauthors = Leventhal H | title = Fear appeals and persuasion: the differentiation of a motivational construct | journal = American Journal of Public Health | volume = 61 | issue = 6 | pages = 1208–1224 | date = June 1971 | pmid = 4110702 | pmc = 1529874 | doi = 10.2105/AJPH.61.6.1208 }}</ref> It also significantly draws from Roger's [[Protection motivation theory]], which proposes two responses to fear-inducing stimuli: threat appraisal and coping appraisal.
The '''extended parallel process model''' ('''EPPM''') is a framework developed by Kim Witte which attempts to predict how individuals will react when confronted with [[fear]] inducing [[Stimulus (psychology)|stimuli]]. It was first published in ''[[Communication Monographs]]'', Volume 59, December 1992; Witte subsequently published an initial test of the model in a later article published in ''[[Communication Monographs]]'' Volume 61, June 1994.
 
The model's main theory is that when confronted with a fear-inducing stimulus, humans tend to engage in two simultaneous ways of message processing: a perceived efficacy appraisal ([[cognitive processing]]) and a perceived threat appraisal (emotional processing). Differences in message appraisal then lead to two behavioural outcomes, with individuals engaging in either a danger control process or a fear control process. In the case of the message being perceived as having no element of threat, individuals do not exhibit a response, and the message is ignored. The EPPM states that the danger control process leads to behavioural change, while the fear control process does not.
EPPM is based on Leventhal's danger control and fear control framework and on Roger's [[protection motivation theory]].<ref>https://www.msu.edu/~wittek/fearback.htm</ref> It is commonly used in health communication campaigns when a message is attempting to persuade audience members to adopt a healthy behavior. Fear-framed campaigns must arouse a moderately-high amount of fear but a higher amount of self-efficacy and response efficacy. When there is more fear than efficacy, the message is expected to not be effective.
 
Witte's EPPM expands on previous fear appeal studies by explaining the reasons for failure in fear appeals and reincorporating fear as a central variable in the model. This is also the first fear appeal model that outlines the relationship between threat and efficacy in propositional forms.
Reviews have highlighted the many applications of the EPPM model in its 20 years since initial publication<ref>{{cite journal|last=Maloney|first=E.|last = Lapinski| first = Maria| last = Witte| first = Kim| year=2011|title=Fear Appeals and Persuasion: A Review and Update ofthe Extended Parallel Process Model|journal=Social and Personality Psychology Compass| volume=5|pages=206-219|doi=10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00341.x}}</ref> but significant theoretical questions on the operationalization of key constructs remain and not all of its hypotheses have received empirical support<ref>{{cite journal|last=Maloney|first=E.|last = Popova| first = L| year=2011|title=
The Extended Parallel Process Model: Illuminating the Gaps in Research| journal=Health Education and Behavior | volume=39|pages=455-473|doi=10.1177/1090198111418108}}</ref>.
 
The EPPM concludes that a fear control process leads to message rejection, while a danger control process leads to message acceptance, leading to adaptive behavioural changes.
==Inputs==
 
== Background ==
[[File:Extended Parallel Process Model.png|400px|thumb|Illustration of the Extended Parallel Process Model.]]
Witte's motivations for designing an updated fear appeal model was due to the declining role of fear in fear appeals. While initially, fear was the pinnacle of theoretical fear appeal literature, it was starting to be considered as a [[control variable]] in subsequent models. A lack of precision in the Parallel Process Model and empirical inconsistencies in the Protection Motivation Theory were also noted by Witte as reasons for formulating an extended parallel process model.
 
Two main components of large-scale public messaging that induce behavioural change are fear appeals and fear appraisals. [[Fear appeal|Fear appeals]] are specifically designed to elicit fear and nudge individuals to adapt to the recommendations in the message. They find their use in public health campaigns and political adverts, and are designed to fit three main categories: message, behaviour, and the audience.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Tannenbaum MB, Hepler J, Zimmerman RS, Saul L, Jacobs S, Wilson K, Albarracín D | title = Appealing to fear: A meta-analysis of fear appeal effectiveness and theories | journal = Psychological Bulletin | volume = 141 | issue = 6 | pages = 1178–1204 | date = November 2015 | pmid = 26501228 | pmc = 5789790 | doi = 10.1037/a0039729 }}</ref>
The EPPM model defines four key factors to predict the likely outcome of communications which involve a fear appeal:
 
* Message: The content that is included in the fear-inducing message
* '''Self-Efficacy''' – The perception the individual has that they are competent to perform the tasks needed to control the risk.
* Behaviour: The behavioural response recommended by the message
* '''Response Efficacy''' – The perception the individual has that the action, if carried out, will successfully control the risk.
* Audience: The characteristics of the audience receiving the message
* '''Susceptibility''' – The perception the individual has of how likely the threat is to impact them.
* '''Severity''' – The perception the individual has of the magnitude of the threat.
 
Fear appraisals are the mental evaluations made in response to experiencing fear-inducing stimuli and are also known as threat appraisals. Fear appeal literature is primarily focused on understanding key fear appraisal processes in humans, with the intention of using it to drive social campaigns and behavioural interventions.<ref name=":1" />
==Outputs==
Based on the inputs above, the EPPM model predicts three possible outcomes.
 
== Components ==
'''Danger control''' – When an individual perceives that the severity and susceptibility are high and also perceives that they are competent to take mitigating action then they are likely to act to control the danger.
The EPPM uses persuasive fear-inducing messages to induce intended behavioural responses. Witte details three main processes involved in fear appraisal: the fear appeal ''inputs,'' the message ''processing'' of the inputs, and the ''outputs'', or action taken after evaluating the perceived threat.
 
=== Fear appeal inputs ===
'''Fear control''' – The model predicts that if an individual perceives their ability to control a risk as low, even if the severity and susceptibility is perceived as high, then they are likely to take steps to control their fear instead. Fear control responses are defined as coping mechanisms that reduce fear and include denial, [[psychological reactance]] and defensive avoidance. These are [[maladaptive]] changes, or counter-productive behaviours. Fear controlling behaviour may involve the use of [[cognitive]] [[defence mechanism]]s such as "It will happen to me sooner or later" in order to manage the state of [[anxiety]].<ref>{{cite journal|last=Maloney|first=E.|last = Lapinski| first = Maria| last = Witte| first = Kim| year=2011|title=Fear Appeals and Persuasion: A Review and Update ofthe Extended Parallel Process Model|journal=Social and Personality Psychology Compass| volume=5|pages=206-219|doi=10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00341.x}}</ref>
[[File:Extended Parallel Process Model.png|400px|thumb|Illustration of the Extended Parallel Process Model.]]According to fear appeal studies, a fear appeal has two components: a component of threat and a component of efficacy. These two components are further divided into two categories each. The threat component is composed of ''severity'' and ''susceptibility'', while the efficacy component is composed of ''response efficacy'' and ''self-efficacy.''
 
The EPPM model definesThese four key factors, toas defined by the EPPM, predict the likely outcome of communications whichthat involve a fear appeal:.
'''No Response''' – The severity or susceptibility of the danger was perceived as low.
 
'''Threat variables'''
==See also==
* [[Behavioural change theories]]
* [[Theory of planned behavior]]
 
* '''Susceptibility''' – The perception the individual has of how likely the threat is to impact them.
==Original articles==
* '''Severity''' – The perception the individual has of the magnitude of the threat.
*Witte, K. (1992). Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model. Communication Monographs, 59(4), 329–349.
*Witte, K. (1994). Fear control and danger control: A test of the extended parallel process model. Communication Monographs, 61(2), 113–134.
 
'''Efficacy variables'''
==References==
{{Reflist}}
 
* '''Self-Efficacy'efficacy'' – The perception the individual has that they are competent to perform the tasks needed to control the risk.
[[Category:Attitude change]]
* '''Response Efficacy'efficacy'' – The perception the individual has that the action, if carried out, will successfully control the risk.
 
The outcome of fear appeals is determined by an ''appraisal'', that is, the evaluation of the message as either dangerous or indifferent.
 
=== Fear appraisal ===
{{psychology-stub}}
[[Appraisal theory|Appraisal Theory]] states that an individual makes either an emotional or affective response to external stimuli. The EPPM outlines two primary appraisals an individual makes in response to a fear appeal: a threat appraisal, followed by an efficacy appraisal.
 
;Low threat appraisal: When a threat appraisal is perceived to be low, i.e., there is a lack of imminent threat, the fear appeal is rejected immediately.
;Moderate to High threat appraisal: When a threat appraisal is perceived as moderate or high, fear is induced, and individuals begin the efficacy appraisal.
;Low efficacy appraisal: When the efficacy appraisal is perceived to be low, the message does not induce behavioural change.
;Moderate to High efficacy appraisal: When the efficacy appraisal is perceived as moderate or high, the message induces a behavioural change.
 
After appraisals of the fear appeal, individuals then take action based on whether the threat is imminent or trivial.
 
=== Fear Appeal Outputs ===
The EPPM predicts three possible outputs after the fear appraisal is carried out:
 
''';Danger control''' –: When an individual perceives that the severity and susceptibility are high (i.e., high threat appraisal) and also perceives that they are competent to take mitigating action (i.e., high efficacy appraisal), then they are likely to act to control the danger.
''';Fear control''' –: The model predicts that if an individual perceives their ability to control a risk as low, even if the severity and susceptibility isare perceived as high, then they are likely to take steps to control their fear instead. Fear control responses are defined as coping mechanisms that reduce fear and include [[denial]], [[psychological reactance]] and [[Avoidance response|defensive avoidance]]. These are [[maladaptive]] changes, or counter-productivecounterproductive behaviours. Fear controlling behaviour may involve the use of [[cognitive]] [[defence mechanism]]s such as "''It will happen to me sooner or later''", in order to manage the state of [[anxiety]].<ref>{{cite journal|last=Maloney|first=E.|last = Lapinski| first = Maria| last = Witte| first = Kim| year=2011|title=Fear Appeals and Persuasion: A Review and Update ofthe Extended Parallel Process Model|journal=Social and Personality Psychology Compass| volume=5|pages=206-219|doi=10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00341.x}}</ref>
''';No Response''' –: The severity or susceptibility of the danger wasis perceived as low, and the individual rejects the message. There is no behavioural change.
 
== Applications ==
The EPPM model is mainly used in [[Social and behavior change communication|social and behaviour change communication]] (SBCC). Practitioners design a general communications program, such as a campaign or an advert, and then test the effectiveness of the program through implementation. SBCC methods in healthcare, education, and marketing have employed the EPPM to induce behavioural change in patients and customers.
 
Multiple versions of the EPPM are employed in health campaigns. For example, EPPM-based campaigns have helped increase colorectal cancer screening participation among young adults<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Birmingham WC, Hung M, Boonyasiriwat W, Kohlmann W, Walters ST, Burt RW, Stroup AM, Edwards SL, Schwartz MD, Lowery JT, Hill DA, Wiggins CL, Higginbotham JC, Tang P, Hon SD, Franklin JD, Vernon S, Kinney AY | display-authors = 6 | title = Effectiveness of the extended parallel process model in promoting colorectal cancer screening | journal = Psycho-Oncology | volume = 24 | issue = 10 | pages = 1265–1278 | date = October 2015 | pmid = 26194469 | pmc = 7161702 | doi = 10.1002/pon.3899 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Pengchit W, Walters ST, Simmons RG, Kohlmann W, Burt RW, Schwartz MD, Kinney AY | title = Motivation-based intervention to promote colonoscopy screening: an integration of a fear management model and motivational interviewing | journal = Journal of Health Psychology | volume = 16 | issue = 8 | pages = 1187–1197 | date = November 2011 | pmid = 21464114 | pmc = 3162074 | doi = 10.1177/1359105311402408 }}</ref> and increased HPV vaccination interest among LatinX.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Reno |first1=Jenna E. |last2=Dempsey |first2=Amanda F. |date=2022-02-18 |title=Promoting HPV vaccination among Latinx: an application of the extended parallel processing model |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-022-00293-7 |journal=Journal of Behavioral Medicine |volume=46 |issue=1–2 |pages=324–334 |language=en |doi=10.1007/s10865-022-00293-7 |pmid=35178652 |s2cid=246905792 |issn=1573-3521|url-access=subscription }}</ref>
 
Other usages of EPPM lie in shaping public perceptions, such as in political adverts, climate change messages, and pandemic responses. <ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = von Gottberg C, Krumm S, Porzsolt F, Kilian R | title = The analysis of factors affecting municipal employees' willingness to report to work during an influenza pandemic by means of the extended parallel process model (EPPM) | journal = BMC Public Health | volume = 16 | issue = 1 | pages = 26 | date = January 2016 | pmid = 26757713 | pmc = 4711035 | doi = 10.1186/s12889-015-2663-8 | doi-access = free }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal| vauthors = Roser-Renouf C, Maibach EW, Leiserowitz A, Zhao X |date=July 2014|title=The genesis of climate change activism: from key beliefs to political action |journal=Climatic Change|language=en|volume=125|issue=2|pages=163–178|doi=10.1007/s10584-014-1173-5|issn=0165-0009|doi-access=free}}</ref>
 
== Criticisms ==
While the EPPM has been effective in health campaigns and behavioural change interventions, there are limitations that have been pointed out through rigorous [[Meta-analysis|meta-analytical]] studies.
 
Reviews have highlighted the many applications of the EPPM model in its 20 years since initial publication <ref>{{citeCite journal|last=Maloney|first=E.|last = Lapinski| firstvauthors = Maria|Maloney lastEK, =Lapinski MK, Witte| firstK |date= Kim|April year=2011 |title=Fear Appeals and Persuasion: A Review and Update oftheof the Extended Parallel Process Model: Fear Appeals and Persuasion |journal=Social and Personality Psychology Compass| language=en|volume=5|issue=4|pages=206-219206–219|doi=10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00341.x}}</ref> but significant theoretical questions on the operationalization of key constructs remain and not all of its hypotheses have received empirical support.<ref name=":0">{{cite journal |last vauthors =Maloney Popova L |first title =E. The extended parallel process model: illuminating the gaps in research |last journal = PopovaHealth Education & Behavior | firstvolume = L39 | yearissue =2011 4 |title pages = 455–473 | date = August 2012 | pmid = 22002250 | doi = 10.1177/1090198111418108 | s2cid = 22928121 }}</ref>
 
Lucy Popova's '''The Extended Parallel Process Model: Illuminating the Gaps in Research''<nowiki/>', is an extensive review on the theoretical and empirical applications of the EPPM. <ref name=":0" /> Popova discovered that the strong theoretical foundations has some inconsistencies in a few of its operational definitions. A systematic review of existing literature on EPPMs found that its propositions had no clear empirical support. The outcomes of fear appeals differ slightly from what the EPPM claims. This questions the practical validity of the EPPM.
 
== See also ==
* [[Social and behavior change communication|Social and behaviour change communication]] – Communication strategies designed to create positive behavioural interventions
* [[Behavioural change theories]] – Theories that attempt to use wide explanations to predict why human behaviours change
* [[Theory of planned behavior]] – The idea that an individual's beliefs shape their behavioural intentions
 
== References ==
{{Reflist|30em}}
 
[[Category:Attitude change]]