Content deleted Content added
Alter: pages. Add: url, doi-access, s2cid. Formatted dashes. | Use this tool. Report bugs. | #UCB_Gadget |
m 1. Format: Reorganised information to make it more readable 2. Word Choice: Changed a few words to make points more concise. 3. Background Information: decided to add 'Communication Monographs' into the introduction section to give readers context. |
||
Line 1:
The '''extended parallel process model''' ('''EPPM''') is a fear appeal theory developed by communications scholar [[Kim Witte]] that illustrates how individuals react to fear-inducing messages.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Witte K | title = Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model. | journal = Communications Monographs | date = December 1992 | volume = 59 | issue = 4 | pages = 329–349 | doi = 10.1080/03637759209376276 }}</ref> Witte subsequently published an initial test of the model in [[Communication Monographs]].<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Witte K | title = Fear control and danger control: A test of the extended parallel process model (EPPM). | journal = Communications Monographs | date = June 1994 | volume = 61 | issue = 2 | pages = 113–134 | doi = 10.1080/03637759409376328 }}</ref>
The EPPM was developed by Witte as a response to the significant inconsistencies in fear appeal literature, serving as an extension of previous fear appeal models, hence the use of 'extended' in name 'EPPM'. The model is originally based on Leventhal's Parallel Process Model – a danger and fear control framework that studied how adaptive protective behaviour stemmed from attempts of danger control.<ref name=":1">{{cite journal | vauthors = Leventhal H | title = Fear appeals and persuasion: the differentiation of a motivational construct | journal = American Journal of Public Health | volume = 61 | issue = 6 | pages = 1208–1224 | date = June 1971 | pmid = 4110702 | pmc = 1529874 | doi = 10.2105/AJPH.61.6.1208 }}</ref> It also significantly draws from Roger's [[Protection motivation theory]], which proposes two responses to fear-inducing stimuli: threat appraisal and coping appraisal.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Rogers RW | title = A Protection Motivation Theory of Fear Appeals and Attitude Change1 | journal = The Journal of Psychology | volume = 91 | issue = 1 | pages = 93–114 | date = September 1975 | pmid = 28136248 | doi = 10.1080/00223980.1975.9915803 }}</ref>
The model's main theory is that when confronted with a fear-inducing stimulus, humans tend to engage in two simultaneous ways of message processing: a perceived efficacy appraisal ([[cognitive processing]]) and a perceived threat appraisal (emotional processing). Differences in message appraisal then lead to two behavioural outcomes, with individuals engaging in either a danger control process or a fear control process. In the case of the message being perceived as having no element of threat, individuals do not exhibit a response, and the message is ignored. The EPPM
Witte's EPPM expands on previous fear appeal studies by explaining the reasons for failure in fear appeals and reincorporating fear as a central variable in the model.
The EPPM concludes that a fear control process leads to message rejection, while a danger control process leads to message acceptance, leading to adaptive behavioural changes.
== Background ==
Witte's motivations for designing an updated fear appeal model was
Two main components of large-scale public messaging that induce behavioural change are fear appeals and fear appraisals. [[Fear appeal|Fear appeals]] are specifically designed to elicit fear and nudge individuals to adapt to the recommendations in the message. They find their use in public health campaigns and political adverts.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Sheeran P, Harris PR, Epton T | title = Does heightening risk appraisals change people's intentions and behavior? A meta-analysis of experimental studies | journal = Psychological Bulletin | volume = 140 | issue = 2 | pages = 511–543 | date = March 2014 | pmid = 23731175 | doi = 10.1037/a0033065 }}</ref> Appeals are designed to fit three main categories: message, behaviour, and the audience.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Tannenbaum MB, Hepler J, Zimmerman RS, Saul L, Jacobs S, Wilson K, Albarracín D | title = Appealing to fear: A meta-analysis of fear appeal effectiveness and theories | journal = Psychological Bulletin | volume = 141 | issue = 6 | pages = 1178–1204 | date = November 2015 | pmid = 26501228 | pmc = 5789790 | doi = 10.1037/a0039729 }}</ref>
Line 21:
== Components ==
The EPPM uses persuasive fear-inducing messages to induce intended behavioural responses. Wittle details three main processes involved in fear appraisal: the fear appeal ''inputs,'' the
=== Fear appeal inputs ===
[[File:Extended Parallel Process Model.png|400px|thumb|Illustration of the Extended Parallel Process Model.]]According to fear appeal studies, a fear appeal has two components: a component of threat and a component of efficacy. These two components are further divided into two categories each. The threat component is composed of ''
These four key factors, as defined by the EPPM, predict the likely outcome of communications that involve a fear appeal '''Threat variables'''
Line 41 ⟶ 43:
[[Appraisal theory|Appraisal Theory]] states that an individual makes either an emotional or affective response to external stimuli. The EPPM outlines two primary appraisals an individual makes in response to a fear appeal: a threat appraisal, followed by an efficacy appraisal.
;Low threat appraisal:
;Moderate to High threat appraisal: When a threat appraisal is perceived as moderate or high, fear is induced, and individuals begin the efficacy appraisal.
;Low efficacy appraisal:
;Moderate to High efficacy appraisal:
After appraisals of the fear appeal, individuals then take action based on whether the threat is imminent or trivial.
Line 56 ⟶ 58:
== Applications ==
The EPPM model is mainly used in [[Social and behavior change communication|
Multiple versions of the EPPM are employed in health campaigns. For example, EPPM-based campaigns have helped increase colorectal cancer screening participation among young adults.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Birmingham WC, Hung M, Boonyasiriwat W, Kohlmann W, Walters ST, Burt RW, Stroup AM, Edwards SL, Schwartz MD, Lowery JT, Hill DA, Wiggins CL, Higginbotham JC, Tang P, Hon SD, Franklin JD, Vernon S, Kinney AY | display-authors = 6 | title = Effectiveness of the extended parallel process model in promoting colorectal cancer screening | journal = Psycho-Oncology | volume = 24 | issue = 10 | pages = 1265–1278 | date = October 2015 | pmid = 26194469 | pmc = 7161702 | doi = 10.1002/pon.3899 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Pengchit W, Walters ST, Simmons RG, Kohlmann W, Burt RW, Schwartz MD, Kinney AY | title = Motivation-based intervention to promote colonoscopy screening: an integration of a fear management model and motivational interviewing | journal = Journal of Health Psychology | volume = 16 | issue = 8 | pages = 1187–1197 | date = November 2011 | pmid = 21464114 | pmc = 3162074 | doi = 10.1177/1359105311402408 }}</ref> Other usages of EPPM lie in shaping public perceptions, such as the adverts on lockdown measures and pandemic protocols during the [[COVID-19 pandemic|Covid-19 Pandemic]].<ref>{{Cite journal| vauthors = Tsoy D, Tirasawasdichai T, Kurpayanidi KI |date=2021 |title=Role of Social Media in Shaping Public Risk Perception during COVID-19 Pandemic: A Theoretical Review |journal=International Journal of Management Science and Business Administration |volume=7|issue=2|pages=35–41|doi=10.18775/ijmsba.1849-5664-5419.2014.72.1005|s2cid=234151462 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Zhao S, Wu X | title = From Information Exposure to Protective Behaviors: Investigating the Underlying Mechanism in COVID-19 Outbreak Using Social Amplification Theory and Extended Parallel Process Model | journal = Frontiers in Psychology | volume = 12 | pages = 631116 | date = 2021 | pmid = 34113280 | pmc = 8185043 | doi = 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.631116 | doi-access = free }}</ref>
== Criticisms ==
While the EPPM has been effective in health campaigns and behavioural change interventions, there are limitations that have been pointed out through rigorous [[Meta-analysis|meta-analytical]] studies.
Reviews have highlighted the many applications of the EPPM model in its 20 years since initial publication <ref>{{Cite journal| vauthors = Maloney EK, Lapinski MK, Witte K |date= April 2011 |title=Fear Appeals and Persuasion: A Review and Update of the Extended Parallel Process Model: Fear Appeals and Persuasion |journal=Social and Personality Psychology Compass|language=en|volume=5|issue=4|pages=206–219|doi=10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00341.x}}</ref> but significant theoretical questions on the operationalization of key constructs remain and not all of its hypotheses have received empirical support.<ref name=":0">{{cite journal | vauthors = Popova L | title = The extended parallel process model: illuminating the gaps in research | journal = Health Education & Behavior | volume = 39 | issue = 4 | pages = 455–473 | date = August 2012 | pmid = 22002250 | doi = 10.1177/1090198111418108 | s2cid = 22928121 }}</ref>
Lucy Popova's '''The Extended Parallel Process Model: Illuminating the Gaps in Research''<nowiki/>', is an extensive review on the theoretical and empirical applications of the EPPM. <ref name=":0" /> Popova discovered that the strong theoretical foundations has some inconsistencies in a few of its operational definitions. A systematic review of existing literature on EPPMs found that its propositions had no clear empirical support.
Line 72 ⟶ 74:
* [[Social and behavior change communication|Social and behaviour change communication]] – Communication strategies designed to create positive behavioural interventions
* [[Behavioural change theories]] – Theories that attempt to use wide explanations to predict why human behaviours change
* [[Theory of planned behavior]] –
== References ==
|