Content deleted Content added
Rathfelder (talk | contribs) removed Category:Health; added Category:Memory processes using HotCat |
m Journal cites:, templated 4 journal cites using AWB (12052) |
||
Line 1:
{{Multiple issues|
{{lead missing|date=December 2015}}
{{Orphan|date=December 2015}}
}}
The [[hippocampus]] participates in the encoding and retrieval of memories.<ref name=a /> The hippocampus is located in the medial temporal lobe (subcortical), and is an infolding of the medial temporal cortex.<ref name=a>Gazzaniga, Michael S., Richard B. Ivry, and G. R. Mangun. "Chapter 9: Memory." Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind. 4th ed. New York: W. W. Norton, 2014. 378-423. Print.</ref> The hippocampus plays an important role in the transfer of information from [[short-term memory]] to [[long-term memory]] during encoding and retrieval stages. These stages need not occur successively, but are, as studies indicate, broadly divided in the neuronal mechanisms they require or even in the hippocampal areas they activate. According to Gazzaniga, “encoding is the processing of incoming information that creates memory traces to be stored.”<ref name=a /> There are two steps to encoding: acquisition and consolidation. During acquisition, stimuli are committed to short term memory.<ref name=a /> Consolidation is where the hippocampus along with other cortical structures stabilize an object within long term memory, a process strengthening over time and one for which a number of theories have arisen to explain.<ref name=a /> After encoding, the hippocampus is capable of going through the retrieval process. The retrieval process consists of accessing stored information; this allows learned behaviors to experience conscious depiction and execution.<ref name=a /> Encoding and retrieval are both affected by [[neurodegenerative]] and [[anxiety disorders]] and [[epilepsy]].
Line 8 ⟶ 10:
===HIPER (Hippocampal Encoding/Retrieval) Model===
Meta-Positron Emission Tomography (PET) analysis has lent support toward a division of the hippocampus between caudal and rostral regions.<ref name=b>{{cite journal | last1 = Lepage
HIPER is a model resulting from and therefore a reflection of certain experimental phenomena, but cannot completely explain hippocampal encoding and retrieval on its own.<ref name=b /> Nevertheless, the model suggests a broad division of labor in encoding and retrieval, whether they involve separate regions of the hippocampus or act simultaneously or independently within a single, more inclusive process.
Line 14 ⟶ 16:
===Theta Phase Separation===
In a framework first developed by Hasselmo and colleagues, theta phase separation implies that the theta rhythm of the hippocampus occurs in cycles and various phases of the rhythm entail encoding and retrieval as separate processes. An extra-hippocampal structure, the septum, initiates and regulates the theta rhythm and its associated memory processes. GABAergic activity within the septum inhibits certain classes of CA3 cells (a region of the hippocampus), the divide often drawn between basket cells, pyramidal cells, and interneurons, to distinguish encoding from retrieval mechanisms. The study emphasizes and models the CA3 subfield of the hippocampus as a primary inducement towards encoding and retrieval. Encoding as a procedure begins when septal GABAergic inhibition is at minimum, freeing basket cells to act within CA3[;], and during brief dis-inhibition periods, other cells receive input: a proximal entorhinal input toward pyramidal cells and a coincident dentate gyrus input toward interneurons.<ref name=c>Kunec, S. (2005). Encoding and Retrieval in the CA3 Region of the Hippocampus: A Model of Theta-Phase Separation. Journal Of Neurophysiology, 94(1), 70-82.
CA3 is significant as it is amiable to auto-associative processes through a recurrent, collateral system.<ref name=c /> The theta phase separation model agrees generally with others on the significance of CA3 but is the first to attribute both the processes of encoding and retrieval to the subfield.<ref name=c />
Line 20 ⟶ 22:
===Reconsolidation Hypothesis===
The reconsolidation hypothesis claims that objects encoded into long term memory experience a new period of consolidation, or the time and resource expended to stabilize a memory object, upon each recollection. This is in opposition to the classical consolidation hypothesis which regards consolidation as a one-time event, following the first encoding of a memory. A memory item in this hypothesis, upon reactivation, destabilizes for a brief period and thereafter invokes the neuronal processes requisite for stabilization.<ref name=d>{{cite journal | last1 = Morris
The reconsolidation hypothesis has lingered since the 1960s; however, a 2000 study, entitled “Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval”, examining fear conditioning in rats, has provided evidence in its favor.<ref name=e>Nader, Karim, Glenn E. Schafe, and Joseph E. Le Doux. 'Fear Memories Require Protein Synthesis In The Amygdala For Reconsolidation After Retrieval'. ''Nature'' 406.6797 (2000): 722-726.
The reconsolidation hypothesis does not suppose that subsequent and precedent consolidation phases are necessarily identical in duration or in the neural mechanisms involved. Nevertheless, the commonality that exists in every consolidation phase is a short-lived destabilization of a memory object and a susceptibility for said object to react to amnesic agents—principally protein synthesis inhibitors.<ref name=d /> Morris and colleagues’ experiment indicates that the reconsolidation hypothesis could apply to particular memory types such as allocentric spatial memory, which is either acquired slowly or rapidly. As implied by the authors, however, such an application is feasible only in the case of rapidly acquired spatial memory, the degree to which is influenced by how thoroughly a spatial object is trained.<ref name=d />
Line 30 ⟶ 32:
===Psychiatric Disorders===
Individuals who develop hippocampal lesions often fare poorly on measures of verbal declarative memory. Tests involving the recall of paragraphs or strings of words, as cited by Bremner and colleagues, illustrate a degree of dysfunction among lesion patients proportionate to the percentage of hippocampal volume and the amount of cells lost.<ref name=f>{{cite journal | last1 = Bremner
As precursors toward later studies that would showcase the effect of [[Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder]] (PTSD) on the human hippocampus, animal studies have broadly demonstrated a susceptibility of the mammalian hippocampus to stressors. In particular, stressed animals develop functional deficits in memory, changes in hippocampal form, and an impairment in neurogenesis, or the ability to produce new neurons.<ref name=f />
Line 40 ⟶ 42:
The effect of seizures on memory are often categorized with respect to their intensity and the cortical areas they affect. Epileptic patients, especially those who suffer from temporal lobe epilepsy, often experience deficits in memory encoding and retrieval, developing anterograde and retrograde amnesia.<ref name=g>Tan, F. (2014) Epilepsy and memory. BC Epilepsy Society, 1-12</ref> At times, if a seizure specifically affects the hippocampus, the individual afflicted can encode memory; however, that memory rapidly extinguishes.<ref name=g />
Accompanying the onset of epilepsies is [[hippocampal sclerosis]], also known as Ammon’s horn sclerosis. Individuals afflicted suffer unilateral volume loss, as evidenced by MRI scans.<ref name=h>Johns, P., Thom, M. (2008) Epilepsy and hippocampal sclerosis: cause or effect? ''Neuropathology'' Article, 8, 16-18</ref> Hippocampal sclerosis involves neural loss and a selective mesial temporal sclerosis (MTS) danger and is likely caused by an overactivation of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) and α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptors by the surplus signaling of excitatory neurotransmitters.<ref name=h /> The depolarization and calcium overload experienced by overactive receptors signal the expression of cell death pathways.<ref name=h />
===Disease===
According to the Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, [[Alzheimer’s]] generally causes a reduction in tissue as well as neurodegeneration throughout the brain. Out of all areas in the brain, the hippocampus is among the first to be damaged by Alzheimer’s. One study located in the Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry tested to see the volume changes of the hippocampus in Alzheimer’s disease patients. Results showed that there was 27% less volume in the hippocampus compared with the hippocampus found in normal cognition. Lastly, the difference between the hippocampus of an Alzheimer’s patient and that of a normal patient was shown through the notable loss seen in cortical grey matter in Alzheimer’s.<ref name=i>Du, A., Schuff, N., Amend, D., Laakso, M., Hsu, Y., Jagust, W., Yaffe, K., Kramer, J., Reed, B., Norman, D., Chui, H., Weiner, M. (2001) Magnetic resonance imaging of the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus in mild cognitive impairment and alzheimer’s disease. ''Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, Psychiatry'', 71, 441-447</ref>
==Experiment==
Line 50 ⟶ 52:
===Methods===
In an experiment performed by Zeineh and colleagues, ten subjects were scanned by fMRI while engaged in a face-name associative task that linked a sequence of faces unknown to the participants with the names of the individuals to whom they belonged.<ref name=j>{{cite journal | last1 = Zeineh
===Results===
|