Content deleted Content added
Line 25:
== History ==
Throughout its history, ensemble coding has taken on many names. Interest in the topic began to emerge in the early 20th century <ref name="Wolfe_2011" />. In its earliest years, ensemble coding was known as [[Gestalt grouping rules|Gestalt grouping]] <ref name="Wolfe_2011" />. In 1923, Max Wertheimer, a [[Gestalt psychology]] theorist, was addressing how
Psychology. London: Routledge and
Line 31:
Kegan Paul Ltd.</ref>. Although Gestaltists helped define some of the central principles of object perception, research into modern ensemble coding did not occur until many years later.
In 1971, [[Norman H. Anderson|Norman Anderson]] was one of the earliest to conduct explicit ensemble coding research <ref name="Haberman_2012" /><ref name=":1">{{Cite journal|last=Anderson|first=Norman H.|date=1971|title=Integration theory and attitude change.|url=http://content.apa.org/journals/rev/78/3/171|journal=Psychological Review|language=en|volume=78|issue=3|pages=171–206|doi=10.1037/h0030834|issn=0033-295X}}</ref>. Anderson's research into social ensemble coding, showed that individuals described by two positive terms were rated more favorably than individuals described by two positive terms and two negative terms <ref name=":0">{{Cite journal|last=Anderson|first=Norman H.|date=1965|title=Averaging versus adding as a stimulus-combination rule in impression formation.|url=http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/h0022280|journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology|language=en|volume=70|issue=4|pages=394–400|doi=10.1037/h0022280|issn=0022-1015}}</ref>. This research on [[impression formation]] demonstrated that a weighted mean or average captures how information is integrated rather than the summation <ref name=":0" />. Additional research during this time explored ensemble coding in group attractiveness <ref>Anderson, N. H., Lindner, R., & Lopes, L.
L. (1973). Integration Theory Applied
Line 45:
Information. Memory &
Cognition, 2(4), 786-790.</ref>, and the perceived badness of criminals <ref>Leon, M., Oden, G. C., & Anderson, N. H.
(1973). Functional Measurement of
Line 56:
== The Current era ==
Consistent with Ariely's findings <ref name="Ariely_2001" />, follow up research conducted by Sang Chul Chong and Anne Treisman in 2003, provided evidence that participants are engaging in summary statistical processes. Their research revealed that participant's maintained high accuracy in encoding the mean size of the stimuli even with short stimuli presentations
Additional work has demonstrated that ensemble coding isn't limited to the mean size of objects in the ensemble <ref name="Ariely_2001" />, but that additional content is extracted like average line orientation <ref name="Dakin_1997">{{cite journal | vauthors = Dakin SC, Watt RJ | title = The computation of orientation statistics from visual texture | journal = Vision Research | volume = 37 | issue = 22 | pages = 3181–92 | date = November 1997 | pmid = 9463699 | doi = 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00133-8 }}</ref>, average spatial ___location <ref name="Alvarez_2008">{{cite journal | vauthors = Alvarez GA, Oliva A | title = The representation of simple ensemble visual features outside the focus of attention | journal = Psychological Science | volume = 19 | issue = 4 | pages = 392–8 | date = April 2008 | pmid = 18399893 | pmc = 2587223 | doi = 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02098.x }}</ref>, average number <ref name="Halberda_2006">{{cite journal | vauthors = Halberda J, Sires SF, Feigenson L | title = Multiple spatially overlapping sets can be enumerated in parallel | journal = Psychological Science | volume = 17 | issue = 7 | pages = 572–6 | date = July 2006 | pmid = 16866741 | doi = 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01746.x }}</ref>, and statistical summaries like the variances <ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Solomon JA, Morgan M, Chubb C | title = Efficiencies for the statistics of size discrimination | journal = Journal of Vision | volume = 11 | issue = 12 | pages = 13 | date = October 2011 | pmid = 22011381 | pmc = 4135075 | doi = 10.1167/11.12.13 }}</ref> are detected. Observers are also able to extract accurate perceptual summaries
== Levels of Ensemble Coding ==
Line 72:
== Social Vision and Ensemble Coding ==
Based on the early work of Anderson <ref name=":1" />, it appears that humans integrate semantic as well as social information using ensemble coding. These findings suggest, that social processes may hinge on the same sort of underlying mechanisms that allow people to perceive average object orientation <ref name="Dakin_1997" /> and average object direction of motion <ref name=":2" /><ref name=":3" /><ref name="Haberman_2012" />.
In recent years, ensemble coding in the field of [[social vision]] has emerged. Social vision is a field of research that examines how people perceive one another. With the addition of ensemble coding, the field is able to explore people perception, or how people perceive groups of other people. This specific research area focuses on how observers accurately perceive and extract social information from groups and how that extracted information influences downstream judgments and behaviors <ref name=":4">{{Cite journal|last=Goodale|first=Brianna M.|last2=Alt|first2=Nicholas P.|last3=Lick|first3=David J.|last4=Johnson|first4=Kerri L.|date=2018-11|title=Groups at a glance: Perceivers infer social belonging in a group based on perceptual summaries of sex ratio.|url=http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/xge0000450|journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: General|language=en|volume=147|issue=11|pages=1660–1676|doi=10.1037/xge0000450|issn=1939-2222}}</ref>. In 2018, seminal research
Additional research has
== References ==
|