Content deleted Content added
ClueBot NG (talk | contribs) m Reverting possible vandalism by 47.4.82.105 to version by Jarble. Report False Positive? Thanks, ClueBot NG. (3791757) (Bot) |
No edit summary |
||
Line 43:
==In middle childhood==
Here we categorize middle childhood as ages 9 to 14. By this stage in a child's normal development vision is sharp and learning processes are well underway. Most studies that have focused their efforts on visual learning have found that visual learning styles as opposed to traditional learning styles greatly improve the totality of a student's learning experience. First off, visual learning engages students and student engagement is one of the most important factors that motivated students to learn. Visuals increase student interest with the use of graphics animation
==In adolescence==
Line 51:
Gray matter is responsible for generating [[nerve impulse]]s that process brain information, and [[white matter]] is responsible for transmitting that brain information between lobes and out through the spinal cord. Nerve impulses are transmitted by [[myelin]], a fatty material that grows around a cell. White matter has a myelin sheath (a collection of myelin) while gray matter doesn't, which efficiently allows neural impulses to move swiftly along the fiber. The myelin sheath isn't fully formed until around ages 24–26.<ref>Wolfe, Pat. (2001). Brain Matters: Translating the Research to Classroom Practice. ASCD: 1-207</ref> This means that adolescents and young adults typically learn differently, and subsequently often utilize [[visual aid]]s in order to help them better comprehend difficult subjects.{{citation needed|date=July 2016}}
Learning preferences can vary across a wide spectrum. Specifically, within the realm of visual learning, they can vary between people who prefer being given learning instructions with text as opposed to those who prefer being given instructions with graphics. College students were tested in general factors like learning preference and [[spatial ability]] (being able to be proficient in creating, holding, and manipulating spatial representations).<ref>Mayer, R. E., & Massa, L. J. (2003). Three Facets of Visual and Verbal Learners: Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Style, and Learning Preference. Journal of educational psychology, 95(4), 833.</ref> The study determined that college-age individuals report efficient learning styles and learning preferences for themselves individually. These personal assessments have proved accurate, meaning that self-ratings of factors such as spatial ability and learning preference can be effective measures of how well one learns visually.{{citation needed|date=July 2016}}
===Gender differences===
Line 67:
|pages= 105–119
|issue=3
|author=Harold Pashler, Mark McDaniel, Doug Rohrer, and Robert Bjork
|year=2009|doi-access=free
}}</ref> Well-designed studies "flatly contradict the popular meshing hypothesis".<ref name=Pashler /> Rather than targeting instruction to the "right" learning style, students appear to benefit most from mixed modality presentations, for instance using both auditory and visual techniques for all students.<ref name=Coffield>Coffield, F., Moseley, D., Hall, E., Ecclestone, K. (2004). [http://www.lsda.org.uk/files/PDF/1543.pdf ''Learning styles and pedagogy in post-16 learning. A systematic and critical review''] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20081205043419/http://www.lsda.org.uk/files/PDF/1543.pdf |date=2008-12-05 }}. London: Learning and Skills Research Centre.</ref>
|