Content deleted Content added
summarize in intro: There is no evidence that providing visual materials to students identified as having a visual style improves learning. |
Srich32977 (talk | contribs) Cleaned up using AutoEd Adding/improving reference(s) |
||
Line 6:
{{undue weight|date=November 2016}}
}}
'''Visual learning''' is a learning style in the [[Learning styles#Neil Fleming's VAK/VARK model|Fleming VAK/VARK model]] where a learner needs to ''see'' information in order to process it. Visual learners can utilize graphs, charts, maps, diagrams, and other forms of visual stimulation to effectively interpret information. The Fleming VAK/VARK model also includes [[kinesthetic learning]] and [[auditory learning]].<ref>Leite, Walter L.; Svinicki, Marilla; and Shi, Yuying: ''Attempted Validation of the Scores of the VARK: Learning Styles Inventory With Multitrait–Multimethod Confirmatory Factor Analysis Models'',
==Techniques==
Line 24:
==Areas of the brain affected==
Various areas of the [[brain]] work together in a multitude of ways in order to produce the images that we see with our eyes and that are encoded by our brains. The basis of this work takes place in the [[visual cortex]] of the brain. The visual cortex is located in the [[occipital lobe]] of the brain and harbors many other structures that aid in visual recognition, categorization, and learning. One of the first things the brain must do when acquiring new visual information is recognize the incoming material. Brain areas involved in recognition are the [[inferior temporal cortex]], the superior [[parietal cortex]], and the [[cerebellum]]. During tasks of recognition, there is increased activation in the left inferior temporal cortex and decreased activation in the right superior parietal cortex. Recognition is aided by [[neural plasticity]], or the brain's ability to reshape itself based on new information.<ref>Poldrack, R., Desmond, J., Glover, G., & Gabrieli, J. "The Neural Basis of Visual Skill Learning: An fMRI Study of Mirror Reading". ''Cerebral Cortex''. Jan/Feb 1998.</ref> Next the brain must categorize the material. The three main areas that are used when categorizing new visual information are the [[orbitofrontal cortex]] and two [[dorsolateral prefrontal]] regions which begin the process of sorting new information into groups and further assimilating that information into things that you might already know.<ref>Vogel, R., Sary, G., Dupont, P., Orban, G. ''Human Brain Regions Involved in Visual Categorization''. Elsevier Science (
==Infancy==
===Where it starts===
Between the fetal stage and 18 months, a baby experiences rapid growth of a substance called [[gray matter]]. Gray matter is the darker tissue of the brain and spinal cord, consisting mainly of nerve cell bodies and branching dendrites.
===The four pathways===
Line 35:
===Supporting studies===
A study by Haith, Hazan, & Goodman in 1988 showed that babies, as young as 3.5 months, are able to create short-term expectations of situations they confront. Expectations in this study refer to the cognitive and perceptual ways in which an infant can forecast a future event. This was tested by showing the infant either a predictable pattern of slides or an irregular pattern of slides and tracking the infant's [[eye movement]]s.<ref>Haith, M. M., Hazan, C., & Goodman, G. S. (1988). "Expectation and Anticipation of Dynamic Visual Events by 3.5 Month Old Babies". ''Child Development'', 59,
A later study by Johnson, Posner, & Rothbart in 1991 showed that by 4 months old, infants can develop expectations, but was tested through anticipatory looks and disengagement with stimuli. For example, anticipatory looks exhibit the infant is able to predict the next part of a pattern which can then be applied to the real world scenario of breast-feeding. Infants are able to predict a mother's movements and expect feeding so they can latch onto the nipple for feeding. Expectations, anticipatory looks, and disengagement all show that infants can learn visually, even if it is only short term.<ref>Johnson, M. H., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1991). "Components of Visual Orienting in Early Infancy: Contingency Learning, Anticipatory Looking, and Disengaing". ''Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience'',
==In early childhood==
From the ages 3–8 visual learning improves and begins to take many different forms. At the toddler age of 3–5, children's bodily actions structure the visual learning environment. At this age, toddlers are using their newly developed sensory-motor skills quite often and fusing them with their improved vision to understand the world around them. This is seen by toddler's using their arms to bring objects of interest close to their sensors, such as their eyes and face, to explore the object further. The act of bringing objects close to their face affects their immediate view by placing their mental and visual attention on that object and just blocking the view of other objects that are around them and out of view. There is an emphasis placed on objects and things that are directly in front of them and thus proximal vision is the primary perspective of visual learning. This is different from how adults utilize visual learning. This difference in toddler vision and adult vision is attributable to their body sizes, and body movements such that their visual experiences are created by their body movement. An adults view is broad, due to their larger body size, with most objects in view because of the distance between them and objects. Adults tend to scan a room, and see everything rather than focusing on one object only.<ref>Smith, L.B., Yu, C., & Pereira, A. F. (2011). "Not your mother's view: The dynamics of toddler visual experience". ''Developmental science'', 14(1),
The way a child integrates visual learning with motor experiences enhances their perceptual and cognitive development.<ref>Bertenthal, B. I., Campos, J. J., & Kermoian, R. (1994). "An epigenetic perspective on the development of self-produced locomotion and its consequences". ''Current Directions in Psychological Science'', 3(5),
==In middle childhood==
Here we categorize middle childhood as ages 9 to 14. By this stage in a child's normal development vision is sharp and learning processes are well underway. Most studies that have focused their efforts on visual learning have found that visual learning styles as opposed to traditional learning styles greatly improve the totality of a student's learning experience. First off, visual learning engages students and student engagement is one of the most important factors that motivated students to learn. Visuals increase student interest with the use of graphics animation and video. Consequently, it has been found that students pay greater attention to lecture material when visuals are used. With increased attention to lesson material, many positive outcomes have been seen with the use of visual tactics in the classrooms of middle-aged students. Students organize and process information more thoroughly when they learn visually which helps them to understand the information better. Students are more likely to remember information that is learned with a visual aid.<ref>Beeland, W. "Student Engagement, Visual Learning, and Technology: Can Interactive Whiteboards Help?" (2001). Theses and Dissertations from Valdosta State University Graduate School.</ref> When teachers used visual tactics to teach middle-aged students they found that students had more positive attitudes about the material they were learning.<ref name="Farkas">Farkas, R. "Effects of Traditional Versus Learning-Styles Instructional Methods on Middle School Students" ''The Journal of Educational Research''. Vol. 97, No. 1 (Sep.
==In adolescence==
Line 50:
===Brain maturation into young adulthood===
Gray matter is responsible for generating [[nerve impulse]]s that process brain information, and [[white matter]] is responsible for transmitting that brain information between lobes and out through the spinal cord. Nerve impulses are transmitted by [[myelin]], a fatty material that grows around a cell. White matter has a myelin sheath (a collection of myelin) while gray matter doesn't, which efficiently allows neural impulses to move swiftly along the fiber. The myelin sheath isn't fully formed until around ages 24–26.<ref>Wolfe, Pat. (2001). "Brain Matters: Translating the Research to Classroom Practice". ''ASCD'':
Learning preferences can vary across a wide spectrum. Specifically, within the realm of visual learning, they can vary between people who prefer being given learning instructions with text as opposed to those who prefer being given instructions with graphics. College students were tested in general factors like learning preference and [[spatial ability]] (being able to be proficient in creating, holding, and manipulating spatial representations).<ref>Mayer, R. E., & Massa, L. J. (2003). "Three Facets of Visual and Verbal Learners: Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Style, and Learning Preference". ''Journal of
===Gender differences===
Studies have indicated that adolescents learn best through 10 various styles; reading, manipulative activity, teacher explanation, auditory stimulation, visual demonstration, visual stimulation (electronic), visual stimulation (just pictures), games, social interaction, and personal experience.<ref>Eiszler, C. F. (1982). "Perceptual Preferences as an Aspect of Adolescent Learning Styles".</ref> According to the study, young adult males demonstrate a preference for learning through activities they are able to manipulate, and young adult females show a greater preference for learning through teacher notes visually or using graphs, and through reading. This suggests that Women are more visually stimulated, interested in information that they can have physical direct control over. Men, on the other hand, learn best through reading information and having it explained in an auditory fashion.
==Lack of evidence==
Line 63:
|issn=1539-6053
|journal=Psychological Science in the Public Interest
|doi= 10.1111/j.1539-6053.2009.01038.x
|pmid=26162104
|volume= 9
|