Content deleted Content added
m Open access bot: doi updated in citation with #oabot. |
Citation bot (talk | contribs) Removed proxy/dead URL that duplicated identifier. | Use this bot. Report bugs. | #UCB_CommandLine |
||
Line 14:
== Working memory ==
Models of working memory primarily focused on declarative memory until Oberauer suggested that declarative and procedural memory may be processed differently in working memory.<ref>{{Cite book|chapter-url=http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S007974210951002X|last=Oberauer|first=Klaus|pages=45–100|doi=10.1016/s0079-7421(09)51002-x|title=The Psychology of Learning and Motivation|volume=51|year=2009|isbn=9780123744890|chapter=Chapter 2 Design for a Working Memory|s2cid=53933457 |url=https://www.zora.uzh.ch/id/eprint/28472/1/Oberauer_PLM_2009.pdf}}</ref> The working memory model is thought to be divided into two subcomponents; one is responsible for declarative, while the other represents procedural memory.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Oberauer|first1=Klaus|last2=Souza|first2=Alessandra S.|last3=Druey|first3=Michel D.|last4=Gade|first4=Miriam|title=Analogous mechanisms of selection and updating in declarative and procedural working memory: Experiments and a computational model|journal=Cognitive Psychology|volume=66|issue=2|pages=157–211|doi=10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.11.001|pmid=23276689|year=2013|s2cid=20150745
==Acquisition of skill==
Line 20:
One model for understanding skill acquisition was proposed by [[Paul Fitts|Fitts]] (1954) and his colleagues. This model proposed the idea that learning was possible through the completion of various stages. The stages involved include:
* Cognitive phase<ref name="fits">{{cite journal | last1 = Fitts | first1 = P. M. | year = 1954 | title = The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement
* Associative phase<ref name="fits"/><ref name="fits2"/>
* Autonomous phase (also called the procedural phase)<ref name="fits"/><ref name="fits2"/>
Line 54:
===Serial reaction time task===
This task involves having participants retain and learn procedural skills that assess specific memory for procedural-motor skill.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Balota | first1 = D.A. | last2 = Connor | first2 = L.T. | last3 = Ferraro | first3 = F.R. | year = 1993 | title = Implicit Memory and the Formation of New Associations in Nondemented Parkinson's Disease Individuals and Individuals with Senile Dementia of the Alzheimer Type: A Serial Reaction Time (SRT) Investigation
===Mirror tracing task===
Line 63:
=== Choice reaction task ===
Choice reaction tasks have been used to assess working memory.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Shahar|first1=Nitzan|last2=Teodorescu|first2=Andrei R.|last3=Usher|first3=Marius|last4=Pereg|first4=Maayan|last5=Meiran|first5=Nachshon|title=Selective influence of working memory load on exceptionally slow reaction times.|journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: General|language=en|volume=143|issue=5|pages=1837–1860|doi=10.1037/a0037190|pmid=25000446|year=2014}}</ref> It has been determined to be useful in gauging procedural working memory by asking participants to follow stimulus-reaction rules.<ref name="Shahar 197–204">{{Cite journal|last1=Shahar|first1=Nitzan|last2=Teodorescu|first2=Andrei R.|last3=Anholt|first3=Gideon E.|last4=Karmon-Presser|first4=Anat|last5=Meiran|first5=Nachshon|title=Examining procedural working memory processing in obsessive-compulsive disorder|journal=Psychiatry Research|volume=253|pages=197–204|doi=10.1016/j.psychres.2017.03.048|pmid=28390295|year=2017|s2cid=13070999
==Expertise==
Line 70:
===Choking under pressure===
It is well established that highly practiced, over-learned skills are performed automatically; they are controlled in real time, supported by procedural memory, require little attention, and operate largely outside of [[working memory]].<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Anderson | first1 = J. R. | year = 1982 | title = Acquisition of a cognitive skill | journal = Psychological Review | volume = 89 | issue = 4| pages = 369–406 | doi=10.1037/0033-295x.89.4.369| s2cid = 18877678
====Rising to the occasion====
If choking on skill-based or co-ordination oriented tasks requires the pressure of the situation to cause the performer's increased conscious attention to his or her process of performance, then the reverse can also be true. A relatively unexplored area of scientific research is the concept of "rising to the occasion." One common misconception is that a person must be an expert in order to have consistent success under pressure. On the contrary, implicit knowledge has been hypothesized to only partially mediate the relationship between expertise and performance.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Otten | first1 = M | year = 2009 | title = Choking vs. Clutch Performance: A Study of Sport Performance Under Pressure
====Famous examples of choking====
Line 87:
== Genetic influence ==
Genetic makeup has been found to impact skill learning and performance, and therefore plays a role in achieving expertise. Using the pursuit rotor task, one study examined the effects of [[Practice (learning method)|practice]] in identical and fraternal twins raised in separate homes. Because identical twins share 100% of their genes while fraternal twins share 50%, the impact of genetic makeup on skill learning could be examined. The results of the pursuit rotor task test became more identical with practice over time for the identical twins, whereas the results for the fraternal twins became more disparate with practice. In other words, the performance of the skill by the identical twins became closer to 100% identical, while the fraternal twins' skill performance became less identical, suggesting the 50% difference in genetic makeup is responsible for the difference in skill performance. The study shows that more practice leads to a closer representation of a person's innate capability, also known as [[Aptitude|talent]]. Therefore, some of the differences people show after extended practice increasingly reflects their genetics. The study also confirmed the idea that practice improves skill learning by showing that, in both the identical and fraternal groups, more practice aided in shedding ineffective tendencies in order to improve execution of a given skill.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Fox|first1=Paul W.|last2=Hershberger|first2=Scott L.|last3=Bouchard|first3=Thomas J.|date=28 November 1996|title=Genetic and environmental contributions to the acquisition of a motor skill|journal=Nature|language=en|volume=384|issue=6607|pages=356–358|doi=10.1038/384356a0|pmid=8934520|bibcode=1996Natur.384..356F|s2cid=4354381
==Anatomical structures==
Line 96:
The [[dorsolateral]] striatum is associated with the acquisition of habits and is the main neuronal cell nucleus linked to procedural memory. Connecting excitatory [[afferent nerve fiber]]s help in the regulation of activity in the basal ganglia circuit. Essentially, two parallel information processing pathways diverge from the striatum. Both acting in opposition to each other in the control of movement, they allow for association with other needed functional structures<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Alexander | first1 = GE | last2 = Crutcher | first2 = MD | year = 1990 | title = Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits; neural substrates of parallel processing | journal = Trends Neurosci | volume = 13 | issue = 7| pages = 266–271 | doi=10.1016/0166-2236(90)90107-l | pmid=1695401| s2cid = 3990601 }}</ref> One pathway is direct while the other is indirect and all pathways work together to allow for a functional neural feedback loop. Many looping circuits connect back at the striatum from other areas of the brain; including those from the emotion-center linked limbic cortex, the reward-center linked [[ventral striatum]] and other important motor regions related to movement.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Haber | first1 = SN | last2 = Fudge | first2 = JL | last3 = McFarland | first3 = NR | year = 2000 | title = Striatonigrostriatal pathways in primates form an ascending spiral from the shell to the dorsolateral striatum | journal = J. Neurosci. | volume = 20 | issue = 6| pages = 2369–2382 | doi = 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-06-02369.2000 | pmid = 10704511 | pmc = 6772499 | doi-access = free }}</ref> The main looping circuit involved in the motor skill part of procedural memory is usually called the cortex-basal ganglia-thalamus-cortex loop.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Parent | first1 = A | year = 1990 | title = Extrinsic connections of the basal ganglia | journal = Trends Neurosci | volume = 13 | issue = 7| pages = 254–258 | doi=10.1016/0166-2236(90)90105-j| pmid = 1695399 | s2cid = 3995498 }}</ref>
The striatum is unique because it lacks the [[glutamate]]-related neurons found throughout most of the brain. Instead, it is categorized by a high concentration of a special type of [[GABA]] related inhibiting cell known as the [[medium spiny neuron]].<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Smith | first1 = Y. | last2 = Raju | first2 = D. V. | last3 = Pare | first3 = J. F. | last4 = Sidibe | first4 = M. | year = 2004 | title = The thalamostriatal system: a highly specific network of the basal ganglia circuitry
Current understanding of brain anatomy and physiology suggests that striatal neural plasticity is what allows basal ganglia circuits to communicate between structures and to functionally operate in procedural memory processing.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Kreitzer | first1 = AC | year = 2009 | title = Physiology and pharmacology of striatal neurons | journal = Annual Review of Neuroscience| volume = 32 | pages = 127–47 | doi=10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135422| pmid = 19400717 }}</ref>
Line 113:
{{further|topic=dopamine|Dopamine}}
[[File:Dopamine Pathways.png|thumb|right|Dopamine Pathways in the brain highlighted in Blue]]
[[Dopamine]] is one of the more known neuromodulators involved in procedural memory. Evidence suggests that it may influence neural plasticity in memory systems by adapting brain processing when the environment is changing and an individual is then forced to make a behavioural choice or series of rapid decisions. It is very important in the process of "adaptive navigation", which serves to help different brain areas respond together during a new situation that has many unknown stimuli and features.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Mizumori | first1 = SJ | last2 = Puryear | first2 = CB | last3 = Martig | first3 = AK | date = Apr 2009 | title = Basal ganglia contributions to adaptive navigation
===At the synapse===
Line 137:
| total_width =
}}
Current Research indicates that procedural memory problems in [[Alzheimer's]] may be caused by changes in enzyme activity in memory-integrating brain regions such as the hippocampus. The specific enzyme linked to these changes is called [[acetylcholinesterase]] (AchE) which may be affected by a genetic predisposition in an immune-system brain receptor called the histamine H1 receptor. The same current scientific information also looks at how [[dopamine]], [[serotonin]] and [[acetylcholine]] neurotransmitter levels vary in the cerebellum of patients that have this disease. Modern findings advance the idea that the [[histamine]] system may be responsible for the cognitive deficits found in Alzheimer's and for the potential procedural memory problems that may develop as a result of the [[psychopathology]].<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Dere | first1 = E. | last2 = Zlomuzica | first2 = A. | last3 = Viggiano | first3 = D. | last4 = Ruocco | first4 = L.A. | last5 = Watanabe | first5 = T. | last6 = Sadile | first6 = A.G. | last7 = Huston | first7 = J.P. | last8 = Souza-Silva | first8 = M.A. De | year = 2008 | title = Episodic-like and procedural memory impairments in histamine H1 Receptor knockout mice coincide with changes in acetylcholine esterase activity in the hippocampus and dopamine turnover in the cerebellum
===Tourette syndrome===
{{further|topic=Tourette syndrome|Tourette syndrome}}
This disease of the central nervous system, like many other procedural-memory related disorders, involves changes in the associated subcortical brain area known as the striatum. This area and the brain circuits closely interacting with it from the basal ganglia are affected both structurally and at a more functional level in the people affected by [[Tourette's syndrome]]. Current literature on this topic provides evidence for there being many unique forms of procedural memory. The one most relevant to procedural memory and most common in Tourette's is related to the skill-acquisition process that ties stimuli to response during the learning part of procedural memory.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Marsh | first1 = R | last2 = Alexander | first2 = GM | last3 = Packard | first3 = MG | last4 = Zhu | first4 = H | last5 = Peterson | first5 = BS | year = 2005 | title = Perceptual-motor skill learning in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Evidence for multiple procedural learning and memory systems
One study has found that those with Tourette syndrome have enhanced procedural learning. Subjects with Tourette's syndrome were found to have more quickly processed procedural knowledge and more accurately learned procedural skills than their typically developed counterparts. Another study found that subjects with Tourette's syndrome displayed faster processing of rule-based grammar than typically developed subjects. Two possible explanations exist for these results. One explanation is that once a person with Tourette's syndrome has learned a procedure, there is a mechanism that supports more accelerated processing. Second, because procedural memory subserves sequencing, and grammar recruits sequencing, an enhancement of grammatical processing was seen in those with Tourette's syndrome due to their improved procedural memories.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Takács | first1 = A | last2 = et | first2 = al. | title = Is procedural memory enhanced in Tourette syndrome? Evidence from a sequence learning task | journal = Cortex | volume = 100 | pages = 84–94 | year = 2017 | doi=10.1016/j.cortex.2017.08.037| pmid = 28964503 | s2cid = 3634434 | url = http://eprints.gla.ac.uk/149676/7/149676.pdf }}</ref>
Line 147:
===Human immunodeficiency virus (HIV)===
{{further|topic=human immunodeficiency virus|HIV}}
Neural systems used by procedural memory are commonly targeted by [[Human Immunodeficiency Virus]]; the striatum being the structure most notably affected.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Reger | first1 = M | last2 = Welsh | first2 = R | last3 = Razani | first3 = J | last4 = Martin | first4 = DJ | last5 = Boone | first5 = KB | year = 2002 | title = A meta-analysis of the neuropsychological sequelae of HIV infection | journal = Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society | volume = 8 | issue = 3| pages = 410–424 | doi=10.1017/s1355617702813212| pmid = 11939699 | s2cid = 30520253 }}</ref> MRI studies have even shown white matter irregularity and basal ganglia subcortical atrophy in these vital areas necessary for both procedural memory and motor-skill.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Chang | first1 = L | last2 = Lee | first2 = PL | last3 = Yiannoutsos | first3 = CT | last4 = Ernst | first4 = T | last5 = Marra | first5 = CM | last6 = Richards | first6 = T | display-authors = etal | year = 2004 | title = A multicenter in vivo proton-MRS study of HIV-associated dementia and its relationship to age
===Huntington's disease===
Line 184:
==Sleep==
Practice is clearly an important process for learning and perfecting a new skill. With over 40 years of research, it is well established in both humans and animals that the formation of all forms of memory are greatly enhanced during the brain-state of sleep. Furthermore, with humans, sleep has been consistently shown to aid in the development of procedural knowledge by the ongoing process of memory consolidation, especially when sleep soon follows the initial phase of memory acquisition.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Karni | first1 = A. | last2 = Tanne | first2 = D. | last3 = Rubenstein | first3 = B.S. | last4 = Askenasy | first4 = J.J. | last5 = Sagi | first5 = D. | year = 1994 | title = Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill | journal = Science | volume = 265 | issue = 5172| pages = 679–682 | doi=10.1126/science.8036518| pmid = 8036518 | bibcode = 1994Sci...265..679K }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Gais | first1 = S. | last2 = Plihal | first2 = W. | last3 = Wagner | first3 = U. | last4 = Born | first4 = J. | year = 2000 | title = Early sleep triggers memory for early visual discrimination skills | journal = Nat. Neurosci. | volume = 3 | issue = 12| pages = 1335–1339 | doi=10.1038/81881| pmid = 11100156 | s2cid = 2075857 | doi-access = free }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Stickgold | first1 = R. | last2 = James | first2 = L. | last3 = Hobson | first3 = J.A. | year = 2000a | title = Visual discrimination learning requires sleep after training | journal = Nat. Neurosci. | volume = 3 | issue = 12| pages = 1237–1238 | doi=10.1038/81756| pmid = 11100141 | doi-access = free }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Stickgold | first1 = R. | last2 = Whidbee | first2 = D. | last3 = Schirmer | first3 = B. | last4 = Patel | first4 = V. | last5 = Hobson | first5 = J.A. | year = 2000b | title = Visual discrimination task improvement: A multi-step process occurring during sleep
Whether a skill is learned explicitly (with [[attention]]) or implicitly, each plays a role in the offline consolidation effect. Research suggests that explicit awareness and understanding of the skill being learned during the acquisition process greatly improves the consolidation of procedural memories during sleep.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Robertson | first1 = E.M. | display-authors = etal | year = 2004 | title = Awareness modifies skill-learning benefits of sleep | journal = Curr. Biol. | volume = 14 | issue = 3| pages = 208–212 | doi=10.1016/s0960-9822(04)00039-9| pmid = 14761652 | doi-access = free }}</ref> This finding is not surprising, as it is widely accepted that intention and awareness at time of learning enhances the acquisition of most forms of memory.
|