Content deleted Content added
format cite |
m clean up, replaced: Journal Of → Journal of (2), Journal of The → Journal of the (2) using AWB |
||
Line 1:
{{multiple issues|
{{Orphan|date=April 2012}}
{{Expert
}}
'''[[Infrasound]]''' is an anthropocentric term that refers to sounds containing some or all energy at frequencies lower than the low frequency end of human hearing threshold at 20 Hz. It is known, however, that humans can perceive sounds below this frequency at very high pressure levels.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Yeowart|first=N. S.|author2=M. J. Evans|title=Thresholds of audibility for very low-frequency pure tones|journal=[[J. Acoust. Soc. Am.]]|year=1974|volume=55|pages=814–818|doi=10.1121/1.1914605}}</ref> Infrasound can come from many natural as well as man-made sources, including weather patterns, topographic features, ocean wave activity, thunderstorms, [[geomagnetic storms]], earthquakes, [[jet streams]], mountain ranges, and rocket launchings.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Cook|first=R. K.|title=Atmospheric sound propagation|journal=Atmospheric
== Mammals ==
Line 16:
==== Infrasound production and perception ====
Recordings and playback experiments support that elephants use the infrasonic components of their calls for communication. Infrasonic vocalizations have been recorded from captive elephants in many different situations. The structure of the calls varies greatly but most of them range in frequency from 14 to 24 Hz, with durations of 10–15 seconds. When the nearest elephant is 5 m from the microphone, the recorded sound pressure levels can be 85 to 90
Playback experiments using prerecorded elephant vocalizations show that elephants can perceive infrasound and how they respond to these stimuli. In playback experiments, certain behaviors that occur commonly after vocalizations are scored before and after a call is played. These behaviors include lifting and stiffening of ears, vocalization, walking or running towards the concealed speaker, clustering in a tight group, and remaining motionless ("freezing"), with occasional scanning movements of the head.<ref name="Langbauer et al 1990" /> The occurrence of such behaviors consistently increases after the playing of a call, whether it is a full-bandwidth playback or a playback in which most of the energy above 25 Hz was filtered out. This filtering shows that the behaviorally significant information of the call is contained in the infrasonic range, and it also simulates the effect of frequency-dependent attenuation over distance as it might occur in the wild.<ref name="Langbauer et al 1990" /> Behavioral responses do not increase for pure tone stimuli that are similar to recorded infrasonic calls in frequency and intensity. This shows that the responses are specifically to signals that were meaningful to the elephants.<ref name="Langbauer et al 1990" />
Line 26:
==== Infrasound sensitivity ====
The auditory sensitivity thresholds have been measured behaviorally for one individual young female Indian elephant. The [[Classical conditioning|conditioning]] test for sensitivity requires the elephant to respond to a stimulus by pressing a button with its trunk, which results in a sugar water reward if the elephant correctly identified the appropriate stimulus occurrence.<ref name="Heffner & Heffner 1980">{{cite journal|last=Heffner|first=H.|author2=R. Heffner|title=Hearing in the elephant (Elephas maximus)|journal=Science|year=1980|volume=208|pages=518–520|doi=10.1126/science.7367876|pmid=7367876|issue=4443}}</ref> To determine auditory sensitivity thresholds, a certain frequency of sound is presented at various intensities to see at which intensity the stimulus ceases to evoke a response. The auditory sensitivity curve of this particular elephant began at 16 Hz with a threshold of
The ability to differentiate frequencies of two successive tones was also tested for this elephant using a similar conditioning paradigm. The elephant’s responses were somewhat erratic, which is typical for mammals in this test.<ref name="Heffner & Heffner 1980" /> Nevertheless, the ability to discriminate sounds was best at frequencies below 1 kHz particularly at measurements of 500 Hz and 250 Hz.<ref name="Heffner & Heffner 1980" />
Line 42:
==== Infrasound sensitivity ====
In experiments using heart-rate conditioning, Pigeons have been found to be able to detect sounds in the infrasonic range at frequencies as low as 0.5 Hz. For frequencies below 10 Hz, the pigeon threshold is at about
In order to use infrasound for navigation, it is necessary to be able to localize the source of the sounds. The known mechanisms for sound localizations make use of the time difference cues at the two ears. However, infrasound has such long wavelengths that these mechanisms would not be effective for an animal the size of a pigeon. An alternative method that has been hypothesized is through the use of the [[Doppler shift]].<ref name="Quine 1981" /> A Doppler shift occurs when there is relative motion between a sound source and a perceiver and slightly shifts the perceived frequency of the sound. When a flying bird is changing direction, the amplitude of the Doppler shift between it and an infrasonic source would change, enabling the bird to locate the source. This kind of mechanism would require the ability to detect very small changes in frequency. A pigeon typically flies at 20 km/hr, so a turn could cause up to a 12% modulation of an infrasonic stimulus. According to response measurements, pigeons are able to distinguish frequency changes of 1-7% in the infrasonic range, showing that the use of Doppler shifts for infrasound localization may be within the pigeon’s perceptive capabilities.<ref name="Quine 1981" />
In early experiments with infrasound sensitivity in pigeons, surgical removal of the calumella, a bone that links the [[tympanic membrane]] to the [[inner ear]], in each ear severely reduced the ability to respond to infrasound, increasing the sensitivity threshold by about
==== Infrasound sensitive nerve fibers ====
Line 60:
== Further reading ==
*Cook, R.K. (1969) Atmospheric sound propagation. Atmospheric exploration by remote probes, Vol. 2, pp. 633–669. Washington, D.C. Committee on Atmospheric Sciences, National Academy of Sciences, National Research Council
*{{cite journal | last1 = Delius | first1 = JD | last2 = Tarpy | first2 = RM | year = 1974 | title = Stimulus control of heart rate by auditory frequency and auditory pattern in pigeons | url = | journal = Journal
*Griffin DR (1969) The physiology and geophysics of bird navigation. ''Q Rev Biol'' 44:255~76
*{{cite journal | last1 = Heffner | first1 = H. | last2 = Heffner | first2 = R. | year = 1980 | title = Hearing in the elephant (Elephas maximus) | url = | journal = Science | volume = 208 | issue = 4443| pages = 518–520 | doi=10.1126/science.7367876 | pmid=7367876}}
*{{cite journal | last1 = Kreithen | first1 = M. L. | last2 = Quine | first2 = D. B. | year = 1979 | title = Infrasound detection by the homing pigeon: A behavioral audiogram | url = | journal = Journal of Physiology A
*{{cite journal | last1 = Langbauer | first1 = W. R. | last2 = Payne | first2 = K. B. | last3 = Charif | first3 = R. A. | last4 = Rapaport | first4 = L. | last5 = Osborn | first5 = F. | year = 1991 | title = African elephants respond to distant playbacks of low-frequency conspecific calls | url = | journal = J. Exp. Biol. | volume = 157 | issue = | pages = 35–46 }}
*{{cite journal | last1 = Langbauer | first1 = W. R. Jr | last2 = Payne | first2 = K. B. | last3 = Charif | first3 = R. A. | last4 = Thomas | first4 = E. M. | year = 1990 | title = Responses of captive African elephants to playback of low-frequency calls | url = | journal = Can. J. Zool. | volume = 67 | issue = | pages = 2604–2607 | doi=10.1139/z89-368}}
*{{cite journal | last1 = Moss | first1 = R | last2 = Lockie | first2 = I | year = 1979 | title = Infrasonic components in the song of the Capercaillie Tetrao urogallus | url = | journal = Ibis | volume = 121 | issue = | pages = 95–97 | doi=10.1111/j.1474-919x.1979.tb05021.x}}
*Payne, Katy. Silent Thunder: In the presence of Elephants. New York: Simon & Schuster, 1998
*{{cite journal | last1 = Payne | first1 = K. B. | last2 = Langbauer | first2 = W. R. | last3 = Jr | first3 = | last4 = Thomas | first4 = E. M. | year = 1986 | title = Infrasonic calls of the Asian elephant (Elephas maximus) | url = | journal = Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. | volume = 18 | issue = | pages = 297–301 | doi=10.1007/bf00300007}}
*{{cite journal | last1 = Quine | first1 = Douglas B. | year = 1981 | title = Frequency shift discrimination: Can homing pigeons locate infrasounds by Doppler shifts? | url = | journal = Journal of Comparative Physiology A | volume = 141 | issue = | page = 2 | doi=10.1007/bf01342661}}
*{{cite journal | last1 = Schermuly | first1 = L. | last2 = Klinke | first2 = R. | year = 1990 | title = Infrasound sensitive neurons in the pigeon cochlear ganglion | url = | journal = Journal of Comparative Physiology A | volume = 166 | issue = | pages = 355–363 | doi=10.1007/bf00204808}}
|