Content deleted Content added
m Robot: fix links to disambiguation page Validity |
Replaced technical language with plain language or added explanations, simplified complex or wordy sentences, reduced excessive quotes by paraphrasing some material, rearranged and reworded sentences to reduce redundancy, removed sentences that were unnecessary, redundant, or did not make sense, reorganized for better flow, added citations. |
||
Line 1:
{{technical|date = December 2010}}
The '''heuristic-systematic model of information processing''', or '''HSM''', is a widely recognized [[communication]] model by [[Shelly Chaiken]] that attempts to explain how people receive and process [[Persuasion|persuasive]] messages. The model states that individuals can process messages in one of two ways: [[heuristics in judgment and decision making|heuristically]] or systematically. Whereas systematic processing entails careful and deliberative processing of a message, heuristic processing entails the use of simplifying decision rules or ‘heuristics’ to quickly assess the message content. The guiding belief with this model is that individuals are more apt to minimize their use of cognitive resources (i.e., to rely on heuristics), thus affecting the intake and processing of messages.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A.|first=|last2=|date=1989|title=Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context|url=|journal=In J. S. Veleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought|publisher=New York: Guilford|volume=|pages=212–252|via=}}</ref> HSM predicts that processing type will influence the extent to which a person is persuaded or exhibits lasting attitude change. HSM is quite similar to the [[elaboration likelihood model]], or ELM. Both models were predominantly developed in the early to mid-
== History ==
Early research investigating how people process persuasive messaging focused mainly on cognitive theories and the way the mind processed
In 1953, [[Carl Hovland|Hovland]], [[Irving Janis|Janis]], and Kelley noted that a sense of "rightness" accompanies holding opinions similar to the opinions of others. In 1987, Holtz and Miller reaffirmed this line of thought by noting, "When other people are perceived to hold similar attitudes, one's confidence in the validity of one's own attitude is increased."<ref name =Petty>Petty, R.E. & Cacioppo, J.T. (1986), Communication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change. New York; Springer-Verlag</ref>
Another concept that contributed to the HSM was the [[sufficiency principle]]. This principle reflected widespread notions that people use limited [[Cognition|cognitive]] resources, or use an "economy-minded" approach to [[information processing]] when presented with persuasive information. Based on this thought, early assumptions said people were at least partially guided by the "[[principle of least effort]]". This principle stated that in the interest of economy, the mind would often process with the least amount of effort ([[heuristic]]), and for more detailed information processing would use more effortful processing (systematic). This was the major difference when compared with the ELM, which described the two different ways information was processed, through central and/or peripheral processing.<ref name=chaiken2>Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology . New York: Guilford Press.</ref>
The developer and main researcher of the HSM was [[Shelly Chaiken]], a now-retired social psychologist. She first received her BS from the [[University of Maryland, College Park]] in 1971 for mathematics. She later earned her MS in 1975 and her PhD in 1978 at the [[University of Massachusetts Amherst]] in [[social psychology]]. In her last position before retiring, Chaiken worked as a professor of psychology at [[New York University]].
Line 18 ⟶ 16:
== Heuristic processing ==
[[Heuristic]] processing uses judgmental rules known as knowledge structures that are learned and stored in memory.<ref name=Chen /> The heuristic approach offers an economic advantage by requiring minimal [[cognitive]] effort on the part of the recipient.<ref name =Chaiken>Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic Versus Systematic Information Processing and the Use of Source Versus Message Cues in Persuasion. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 39(5), 752-766. Retrieved from SocINDEX database.</ref> Heuristic processing is related to the concept of "[[satisficing]]."<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Simon|first=Herbert A.|date=1955-2|title=A Behavioral Model of Rational Choice|url=https://academic.oup.com/qje/article-lookup/doi/10.2307/1884852|journal=The Quarterly Journal of Economics|volume=69|issue=1|pages=99|doi=10.2307/1884852}}</ref>
Heuristic processing is governed by availability, accessibility, and applicability. Availability refers to the knowledge structure, or heuristic, being stored in memory for future use. Accessibility of the heuristic applies to the ability to retrieve the memory for use. Applicability of the heuristic refers to the relevancy of the memory to the judgmental task.<ref name="Chen">Chen, S., Duckworth, K., & Chaiken, S. (1999). Motivated Heuristic and Systematic Processing. Psychological Inquiry, 10(1), 44. Retrieved from SocINDEX database</ref> Due to the use of knowledge structures, a person using heuristic information Individuals may be more likely to use heuristic processing when an issue is less personally important to them (they have low “issue involvement”) or when they believe their judgment will not have significant impacts on themselves (low “response involvement”).<ref name="Chaiken" />
== Systematic processing ==
Systematic processing involves comprehensive and analytic, cognitive processing of judgment-relevant information.<ref name=Chen /> The systematic approach values source [[Reliability (statistics)|reliability]] and message content, which may exert stronger impact on persuasion, when determining message validity.<ref name=Chaiken /> Judgments developed from systematic processing rely heavily on in-depth treatment of judgment-relevant information and respond accordingly to the semantic content of the message.<ref name=Chen /> Recipients developing attitudes from a systematic basis exert considerable cognitive effort and actively attempt to comprehend and evaluate the message's arguments.
== Choosing systematic or heuristic processing ==
Line 28 ⟶ 30:
Both heuristic and systematic processes may occur independently. It is also possible for both to occur simultaneously in an additive fashion or in a way that the judgmental implications of one process lend a bias nature to the other.<ref name=Chen /> The heuristic-systematic model includes the hypothesis that attitudes developed or changed by utilizing heuristic processing alone will likely be less stable, less resistant to counterarguments, and will be less predictive of subsequent behavior than attitudes developed or changed utilizing systematic processing.<ref name=Chaiken />
Source credibility affects persuasion under conditions of low, but not high, issue-involvement and response-involvement.<ref name="Chaiken" />
When
== Practical application ==
Research into information processing, especially in persuasive messaging, has a natural application in advertising, specifically medical awareness. A 2004 study by Suzanne K. Steginga, PhD, and Stefano Occhipinti, PhD, [[Queensland Cancer Fund]] and the School of Applied Psychology, [[Griffith University]], [[Queensland]], [[Australia]], investigated the utility of the heuristic-systematic processing model as a framework for the investigation of patient decision making. A total of 111 men diagnosed with localized prostate cancer were assessed using [[verbal protocol analysis]] and self-report measures. The results showed: "Most men (68%) preferred that decision making be shared equally between them and their doctor. Men's use of the expert opinion heuristic was related to men's verbal reports of decisional uncertainty and having a positive orientation to their doctor and medical care; a desire for greater involvement in decision making was predicted by a high internal locus of health control. Trends were observed for systematic information processing to increase when the heuristic strategy used was negatively [[affect (psychology)|affect]]-laden and when men were uncertain about the probabilities for cure and side effects. There was a trend for decreased systematic processing when the expert opinion heuristic was used. Findings were consistent with the heuristic-systematic processing model and suggest that this model has utility for future research in applied decision making about health issues.<ref name =app>{{cite journal|authors=Steginga, Suzanne K.; Occhipinti, Stefano|url=http://mdm.sagepub.com/content/24/6/573.short|title=The Application of the Heuristic-Systematic Processing Model to Treatment Decision Making about Prostate Cancer|journal=Med Decis Making|year= 2004|volume=24|number=6|pages=573–583|doi=10.1177/0272989X04271044|pmid=15534339}}</ref>▼
▲
▲The model is also used in Internet webpage considerations. In a 2002 study by Wathen & Burkell, they proposed a theory that separated the evaluation process into distinct segments. In the theory, the process began with low-effort examinations of peripheral cues (e.g., appearance, design, organization, and source reputation) then continued to a more high-effort analysis of the content of the information source. The proposed research also drew on social psychological theories of dual-processing, which stated that information processing outcomes were the result of interaction between a fast, associative information-processing mode based on low-effort heuristics, and a slow, rule-based information processing mode based on high-effort systematic reasoning. Wathen and Burkell proposed (but did not test) that if an individual determines that an online source does not meet an appropriate level of credibility at any one stage, then he or she will leave the site without further evaluation. They theorized that this “easy to discard” behavior was indicative of information-rich environments, where the assumption is that many other potential sources of information exist, and spending too much time on any one source is potentially wasteful.<ref name=wathen>Wathen, C. N., & Burkell, J. (2002). Believe it or not: Factors influencing credibility on the Web. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 53(2), 134–144</ref>
== Direction of future research ==
Originally the heuristic-systematic model was developed to apply to "validity seeking" [[persuasion]]
* Defense-motivation is the desire to form or defend particular attitudinal positions.
Line 54 ⟶ 55:
== Criticisms ==
A major criticism of HSM is that the model closely relates to [[Elaboration likelihood model|ELM]], which is also a dual-processing model discussing two main paths to [[persuasion]]. The
In ELM, the central route is reflective and requires a willingness to process and think about the message. The peripheral route occurs when attitudes are formed without extensive thought, but more from mental shortcuts, credibility, and appearance cues. The route of persuasion processing depends on the level of involvement in the topic or issue. High involvement or elaboration increases central route processing especially when motivation and ability in the message exists. Therefore, low involvement increases peripheral route processing when motivation and ability conditions of persuasion do not exist. However, if the topic or idea is irrelevant to the individual, then the message takes the peripheral route.<ref name HSM specifically examines
Major assumptions exist with both HSM and ELM, which is why both models have generated debate and are often misconstrued. Systematic processing assumes that persuasion has
This leads to another similarity between HSM and ELM, as attitudes and opinions developed through heuristic processing will tend to be "less stable, less resistant to counter-propaganda, and less predictive of behavior" in comparison to attitudes and opinions formed through detailed information within systematic processing (p. 327).<ref name=Eagly />
== See also ==
|