Ensemble coding: Difference between revisions

Content deleted Content added
mNo edit summary
No edit summary
Line 66:
 
=== Low-level feature information ===
Low-level ensemble coding has been observed in various psychophysical areas of research. For example, people accurately perceive the average size of objects <ref name="Chong_2003" />, motion direction of grouped dots <ref name=":2">{{cite journal | vauthors = Watamaniuk SN, Sekuler R, Williams DW | title = Direction perception in complex dynamic displays: the integration of direction information | journal = Vision Research | volume = 29 | issue = 1 | pages = 47–59 | date = 1989-01-01 | pmid = 2773336 | doi = 10.1016/0042-6989(89)90173-9 }}</ref><ref name=":3">{{cite journal | vauthors = Watamaniuk SN, McKee SP | title = Simultaneous encoding of direction at a local and global scale | journal = Perception & Psychophysics | volume = 60 | issue = 2 | pages = 191–200 | date = February 1998 | pmid = 9529903 | doi = 10.3758/BF03206028 }}</ref>, number <ref name="Halberda_2006" />, line orientation <ref name="Dakin_1997" />, and spacial ___location <ref name="Alvarez_2008" /><ref name="Whitney_2014" />.
 
=== High-level feature information ===
Line 72:
 
== Social Vision Ensemble Coding ==
Based on the early work of Anderson<ref name=":1" />, it appears that humans integrate semantic as well as social information using ensemble coding. "SocialThese perceptionsfindings suggest, that andsocial attitudesprocesses may hinge on the same sort of underlying computations and mechanisms that allow uspeople to perceive average object orientation<ref name="Dakin_1997" and/>, direction of motion<ref name=":2" /><ref name=":3" /><ref name="Haberman_2012" />. Social ensemble coding [[social vision]].
 
== References ==