Memory development: Difference between revisions

Content deleted Content added
I have added some "needed citations" to the wikipedia article that was formerly done. I hope they are correct! I have also some new information in most categories in this articles, I have also added their respective resources in citations. Some of them were a bit difficult to find, I hope it is correct. Please let me know if there is anything that needs to be changed, or you need to change yourself so that it can be more accurate and make sure its not plagiarized.
Fix cite date error
Line 1:
{{short description|Development of memory in children}}
The '''development of memory in children''' becomes evident within the first 3 years of a child's life as they show considerable advances in [[declarative memory]]. An important aspect to reckon throughout this article, is that the memory of a child ameliorates throughout their development.<ref name="Learning & memory">{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|title=Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}</ref> As mentioned previously this enhancement continues into adolescence with major developments in [[short term memory]], [[working memory]], [[long term memory]] and [[autobiographical memory]].<ref name=bad>{{cite book|last=Siegler|first=R. S.|title=Children's Thinking|year=1998|publisher=Prentice Hall|___location=Upper Saddle River, NJ|edition=3rd|authorlink=Robert S. Siegler|isbn=978-0-13-397910-7}}{{pnpage needed|date=August 2016}}</ref>
 
Recent research on the development of memory has indicated that declarative, or explicit memory, may exist in infants who are even younger than two years old. For example, newborns who are less than 3 days old demonstrate a preference for their mother’s own voice, as well as proposing the importance of a strong connection to the mother.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=DeCasper |first1=A. |last2=Fifer |first2=W. |title=Of human bonding: newborns prefer their mothers' voices |journal=Science |volume=208 |issue=4448 |pages=1174–6 |year=1980 |pmid=7375928 |doi=10.1126/science.7375928 |bibcode=1980Sci...208.1174D |citeseerx=10.1.1.553.1738 }}</ref>
Line 12:
 
==Working Memory==
According to [[Baddeley's model of working memory]], working memory is composed of three parts. First is the central [[Executive (management)|executive]] which is responsible for a range of regulatory functions including attention, the control of action, and problem solving. Second, the [[phonological loop]], which is specialized for the manipulation and retention of material in particular informational domains. Finally, the visuospatial sketchpad stores material in terms of its visual or spatial features. The strength of the relationships between the three components of working memory vary; the central executive is strongly linked with both the phonological loop as well as the visuospatial sketchpad which are both independent of each other. Some evidence indicates linear increases in performance of working memory from age 3-43–4 years through to adolescence.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Baddeley |first1=A. D. |last2=Hitch |first2=G. |year=1974 |chapter=Working memory |editor1-first=G. |editor1-last=Bower |title=The psychology of learning and motivation |volume=8 |pages=47–90 |___location=New York |publisher=Academic Press }}</ref><ref>{{Cite book | url=https://books.google.com/?id=MB_PBwAAQBAJ&pg=PA218&lpg=PA218&dq=The+development+of+a+cognitive+self+is+also+thought+by+some+to+have+an+effect+on+encoding+and+storing+early+memories#v=onepage&q=The%20development%20of%20a%20cognitive%20self%20is%20also%20thought%20by%20some%20to%20have%20an%20effect%20on%20encoding%20and%20storing%20early%20memories&f=false | title=Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, Cognitive Processes| isbn=9781118953846| date=2015-03-31}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Eacott |first1=Madeline J. |title=Memory for the Events of Early Childhood |journal=Current Directions in Psychological Science |volume=8 |issue=2 |year=1999 |pages=46–8 |jstor=20182556 |doi=10.1111/1467-8721.00011 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Bauer |first1=Patricia J. |last2=Larkina |first2=Marina |title=Childhood amnesia in the making: Different distributions of autobiographical memories in children and adults |journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: General |volume=143 |issue=2 |year=2014 |pages=597–611 |doi=10.1037/a0033307 |pmid=23937179 }}</ref><ref name="DOI 10.1075/ce.4.2.02jos">{{cite journal |last1=Joseph |first1=Rhawn |title=Emotional trauma and childhood amnesia |journal=Consciousness & Emotion |volume=4 |issue=2 |year=2003 |pages=151–79 |doi=10.1075/ce.4.2.02jos }}</ref>
 
===Central Executive===
Central executive is an important integral of the working memory, in where it has the all- inclusive attentional control of the working memory system. <ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref> Towards the beginning it was inferred that the central executive had an important role of storing some information. It was also inferred that the central executive reinforced long-term memory, as well as having the potential to designate resources for focusing, dividing and switching attention. <ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref> Currently the model of the central executive excludes the possibility of any type of memory storage. However, it does include the new understanding that it does have a responsibility for the control and reinforcement of attention.<ref>{{Cite book|last=Baddeley, Alan D., 1934-|url=http://worldcat.org/oclc/2118601|title=The psychology of memory|date=1976|publisher=Basic Books|isbn=0-465-06736-0|oclc=2118601}}</ref><ref>{{Citation|last=Baddeley|first=Alan|title=What limits working memory span?|date=2007-03-15|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198528012.003.0011|work=Working Memory, Thought, and Action|pages=189–210|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn=978-0-19-852801-2|access-date=2020-03-30}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last=Baddeley|first=Alan|date=November 2000-11|title=The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory?|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01538-2|journal=Trends in Cognitive Sciences|volume=4|issue=11|pages=417–423|doi=10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01538-2|issn=1364-6613}}</ref><ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref> In children from 2-4, the memory storage capacity limitation constrains complex comprehension processes. As the child grows older however, less processing is necessary which opens more storage space for memory.<ref name="gather">{{cite journal |last1=Gathercole |first1=Susan E. |title=The Development of Memory |journal=Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry |volume=39 |issue=1 |pages=3–27 |year=1998 |pmid=9534084 |doi=10.1111/1469-7610.00301 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Eacott |first1=M. J. |last2=Crawley |first2=R. A. |title=The offset of childhood amnesia: Memory for events that occurred before age 3 |journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: General |volume=127 |issue=1 |year=1998 |pages=22–33 |doi=10.1037/0096-3445.127.1.22 |pmid=9503650}}</ref><ref name=pmid17654279>{{cite journal |last1=Jack |first1=Fiona |last2=Hayne |first2=Harlene |title=Eliciting adults' earliest memories: Does it matter how we ask the question? |journal=Memory |volume=15 |issue=6 |pages=647–63 |year=2007 |pmid=17654279 |doi=10.1080/09658210701467087 }}</ref>
 
===Phonological Loop===
Line 24:
 
==Long Term Memory==
Long term memory, also known as episodic and semantic memory, has the ability to store valuable information for a proficient amount of time.<ref name="worldcat.org">{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/941831058|title=The Gale encyclopedia of psychology|others=Longe, Jacqueline L.,|isbn=978-1-4103-1781-0|edition=Third edition|___location=Farmington Hills, MI|oclc=941831058}}</ref> According to Longe (2016) the storage of long-term memory could be in assortments of minutes to lifetime, meaning an activity or event attended can be recalled after a few minutes or be stored for a long time.<ref>{{Cite book|urlname=https://www."worldcat.org"/oclc/941831058|title=The Gale encyclopedia of psychology|others=Longe, Jacqueline L.,|isbn=978-1-4103-1781-0|edition=Third edition|___location=Farmington Hills, MI|oclc=941831058}}</ref> Long term memory uses a very important distinguishing factor known as meaning that can help an individual learn: It is used in a form of encoding and it is deemed the primary method of developing long-term memory. <ref>{{Cite book|urlname=https://www."worldcat.org"/oclc/941831058|title=The Gale encyclopedia of psychology|others=Longe, Jacqueline L.,|isbn=978-1-4103-1781-0|edition=Third edition|___location=Farmington Hills, MI|oclc=941831058}}</ref> Once meaning is understood and applied to information it can impact what one recalls. <ref>{{Cite book|urlname=https://www."worldcat.org"/oclc/941831058|title=The Gale encyclopedia of psychology|others=Longe, Jacqueline L.,|isbn=978-1-4103-1781-0|edition=Third edition|___location=Farmington Hills, MI|oclc=941831058}}</ref>
 
Explicit memory becomes much better over the developmental years. However, there are small effects of age on implicit memory, which could be because implicit memory involves more basic processes than declarative memory which would make it less affected by a child's developing cognitive skills and abilities. <ref>{{Cite journal|last=Ward|first=Emma V.|last2=Berry|first2=Christopher J.|last3=Shanks|first3=David R.|date=2013|title=Age effects on explicit and implicit memory|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00639|journal=Frontiers in Psychology|volume=4|doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00639|issn=1664-1078}}</ref>
 
===Infants===
It has been recorded from numerous studies that infants with the early age of 2 to 3 months can in develop a memory from certain experiences. <ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref> A surprising finding was that within the same age group of 2 to 3 months, infants could also remember an event or memory that was forgotten over the years. <ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref> The infant experienced this recollection by accosting a certain factor that might have sparked that forgotten memory. <ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref> These impressive findings were found by testing the kicking of infants. According to Robert & Saweikis (2004), researchers placed a mobile over the infant’s crib and a ribbon that connects the infant’s leg to the mobile. <ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref> The infants demonstrated to the researchers that they were learning the connection between their kicking and the mobile’s movement.<ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref> Once the allotted time passed, the infant’s leg was attached once more to the mobile. <ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref> Two types of ideas were formed; supposing that the child could energetically start kicking, would lead to the assumption that the infant remembered the connection between the mobile’s movement and the childs kicking. Now, if the infant’s kicking gingerly become more energetic, that would presume that the infant is relearning the connection, which would suggest that the infant has forgotten the connection made. <ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref>
 
The study also indicated that the infant could remember the connection for up to 14 days. <ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref> However, once certain time has passed the infant’s leg is once again connected to the mobile’s movement with a ribbon to test of the infant recalled what to do. <ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref> The infant did not remember what to do, and they were introduced to a certain factor that would aid the infant to remember. <ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref> According to Robert & Saweikis (2004), If the “experimenter” moves the mobile showing the infant the movements, as soon as the infant is reconnected to the mobile with a ribbon, the infant will start kicking energetically. <ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref> The conclusion was that the infant could indeed remember a memory, although time has passed.<ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref>
 
Infants who are 5 months or older are able to use emotions to influence their memories. However, at this age, infants will be more likely to remember things that were characterized by positive emotions. Numerous mechanisms that are used to study and infer memory in children cannot be used on infants, due to the process the study is retrieved, which include writing or speaking. <ref>{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/49977789|titlename="Learning & memory|date=2003|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|others=Byrne, John H.|isbn=0-02-865619-9|edition=2nd ed|___location=New York|oclc=49977789}}<"/ref> The way that researchers study the memory capabilities of infants in this age range is through measuring eye movements between test images presented. After doing this initial round of testing, the researchers would conduct follow-up tests both 5 minutes later and one day later. The follow-up tests shown to the infants included two geometric shapes: one from the original test, and a new shape. The researchers were able to record how long the infants looked at the images in the follow-up tests and measured how long the infants stared at each shape. The infants were more likely to gaze at the geometric shapes from the original tests if they had been paired with positive voices than if they had been paired with neutral or negative voices. This study indicated that infants at this age would be able to better remember shapes and patterns of things if they were associated with positive emotions because positivity would increase the infants' interest and attention.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Flom |first1=Ross |last2=Janis |first2=Rebecca B. |last3=Garcia |first3=Darren J. |last4=Kirwan |first4=C. Brock |title=The effects of exposure to dynamic expressions of affect on 5-month-olds' memory |journal=Infant Behavior and Development |volume=37 |issue=4 |pages=752–9 |year=2014 |pmid=25459793 |doi=10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.09.006 }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last=Sullivan|first=Margaret Wolan|last2=Lewis|first2=Michael|date=April 2003-04|title=Emotional Expressions of Young Infants and Children: A Practitionerʼs Primer|url=http://journals.lww.com/00001163-200304000-00005|journal=Infants & Young Children|language=en|volume=16|issue=2|pages=120–142|doi=10.1097/00001163-200304000-00005|issn=0896-3746}}</ref>
 
===Pre-school Children===
Infants at as early as 7-months-old can conceptually differentiate between categories such as animals and vehicles. Although infants’ concepts may be crude by adult standards, they still allow infants to make meaningful semantic distinctions. An example is that infants can differentiate between items belonging to a kitchen and those items belonging to a bathroom.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Mandler |first1=Jean M. |last2=Fivush |first2=Robyn |last3=Reznick |first3=J. Steven |title=The development of contextual categories |journal=Cognitive Development |volume=2 |issue=4 |year=1987 |pages=339–54 |doi=10.1016/S0885-2014(87)80012-6 }}</ref> At the very least, these categories lay a foundation for early knowledge development, organizing information in storage and influence future encoding. Infants from 16 months old are able to draw on their semantic knowledge in [[generalization]] and [[inference]]. This knowledge can also be used by older toddlers, 24-month-olds, to facilitate acquisition and retention of new information. Their knowledge of causal ordering of events can be used to help to recall the sequence of events.<ref>{{Cite book|last=Henry, Lucy.|url=http://worldcat.org/oclc/926809576|title=The development of working memory in children|date=2011|publisher=SAGE|isbn=978-1-84787-329-3|oclc=926809576}}</ref> Infants have the ability to recall experiences after some time or demonstrate that they have a forming cognitive process. <ref name="Henry, Lucy. 2012">{{Cite book|last=Henry, Lucy.|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/797817779|title=The development of working memory in children|date=2012|publisher=SAGE|isbn=978-1-4462-5419-6|___location=Los Angeles|oclc=797817779}}</ref><ref name="Flavell 324–340">{{Cite journal|last=Flavell|first=John H|last2=Friedrichs|first2=Ann G|last3=Hoyt|first3=Jane D|date=October 1970-10|title=Developmental changes in memorization processes|url=https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/0010028570900198|journal=Cognitive Psychology|language=en|volume=1|issue=4|pages=324–340|doi=10.1016/0010-0285(70)90019-8}}</ref>
 
Pre-school children can be heavily inaccurate in recalling words or numbers they have just learned. <ref>{{Cite journal|lastname="Flavell|first=John H|last2=Friedrichs|first2=Ann G|last3=Hoyt|first3=Jane D|date=1970-10|title=Developmental changes in memorization processes|url=https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/0010028570900198|journal=Cognitive Psychology|language=en|volume=1|issue=4|pages=324–340|doi=10.1016"/0010-0285(70)90019-8}}</ref> <ref>{{Cite book|lastname="Henry, Lucy.|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/797817779|title=The development of working memory in children|date=2012|publisher=SAGE|isbn=978-1-4462-5419-6|___location=Los Angeles|oclc=797817779}}<"/ref> Children are more able to recall information, which according to Henry (2011) children can “predict” memory performance if they have online experience with a task.<ref>{{Cite book|lastname="Henry, Lucy.|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/797817779|title=The development of working memory in children|date=2012|publisher=SAGE|isbn=978-1-4462-5419-6|___location=Los Angeles|oclc=797817779}}<"/ref> What lead to this conclusion was the children were given a a tape recorder with 10 words, the kids were asked to stop the tape recorder once they thought they could remember all the words mentioned. <ref>{{Cite book|lastname="Henry, Lucy.|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/797817779|title=The development of working memory in children|date=2012|publisher=SAGE|isbn=978-1-4462-5419-6|___location=Los Angeles|oclc=797817779}}<"/ref> According to the study 17% children predicted that they knew all 10 words mentioned. <ref>{{Cite book|lastname="Henry, Lucy.|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/797817779|title=The development of working memory in children|date=2012|publisher=SAGE|isbn=978-1-4462-5419-6|___location=Los Angeles|oclc=797817779}}<"/ref>
 
Knowledge itself will not alter retention performance, rather how well that knowledge is structured will alter performance. Better retention was shown with information that had greater cohesion and more elaborative elements. Familiarity and repetition of an experience can also influence the organization of information in storage for preschoolers and older children.<ref name="fivush" /> Children who experienced an event twice recalled the event better 3 months later than did children who only experienced it once and showed equally good recall at 3 months compared to recall at 2 weeks after experiences.<ref name="swrtc.nmsu.edu">{{Cite web|url=https://swrtc.nmsu.edu/cli-2/pastconferences/2017-2/agenda/childtraumatic/|title=Child Traumatic Memory and the Testimony of Children {{!}} Southwest Regional Training Center {{!}} New Mexico State University|language=en|access-date=2020-03-30}}</ref>
 
===School Age Children===
Age differences in memory are attributed to age-correlated growth in the foundation of knowledge. What children know affects what they encode, how that information is organized in storage, and the manner in which it’s retrieved. The greater the background knowledge about the to-be-encoded information, the better that the information is remembered.<ref name="fivush" /> Because older children have more knowledge than younger children, older children perform better than younger children in most memory tasks. When familiarity and meaningfulness of material were equated across age, developmental differences in memory performance was no longer a factor.<ref>{{Cite web|urlname=https://"swrtc.nmsu.edu"/cli-2/pastconferences/2017-2/agenda/childtraumatic/|title=Child Traumatic Memory and the Testimony of Children {{!}} Southwest Regional Training Center {{!}} New Mexico State University|language=en|access-date=2020-03-30}}</ref>
 
Children’s use of memory strategies and the development of metamemory skills are also instrumental in age-related changes in memory, particularly later in childhood years.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=O’Sullivan |first1=Julia T. |last2=Howe |first2=Mark L. |title=A different view of metamemory with illustrations from children's beliefs about long-term retention |journal=European Journal of Psychology of Education |volume=13 |issue=1 |year=1998 |pages=9–28 |jstor=23420104 |doi=10.1007/BF03172810 }}</ref> Knowledge influences memory by affecting retrieval, by facilitating spread of activation among related items in memory and by facilitating the use of strategies. Knowledge also provides better elaboration of information which can strengthen its storage in memory.<ref>{{Cite book|lastname="Henry, Lucy.|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/797817779|title=The development of working memory in children|date=2012|publisher=SAGE|isbn=978-1-4462-5419-6|___location=Los Angeles|oclc=797817779}}<"/ref>
 
==Episodic Memory==
Line 53:
By school age, the typical child shows skill in recalling details of past experiences and in organizing those details into a [[narrative]] form with [[:wikt:cohesion|cohesion]]. Memories formed at this age and beyond are more likely to stand the test of time over the years and be recalled in adulthood, compared to earlier memories. Young children can sometimes retain information from specific episodes over very long periods of time, but the particular information a child of a particular age is likely to retain over different periods of time is unpredictable. This depends on the nature of the memory event and individual differences in the child such as gender, parental style of communication, and language ability.<ref name="fivush">{{cite journal |last1=Fivush |first1=Robyn |last2=Gray |first2=Jacquelyn T. |last3=Fromhoff |first3=Fayne A. |title=Two-year-old talk about the past |journal=Cognitive Development |volume=2 |issue=4 |year=1987 |pages=393–409 |doi=10.1016/S0885-2014(87)80015-1 }}</ref>
 
One of the most important aspects of episodic memory according to Tulving (1985, 1999) is the element of the individual to cognitively travel to both the past and the future.<ref name="John Wiley & Sons Ltd">{{Cite book|url=http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/9781118597705|title=The Wiley Handbook on the Development of Children's Memory: Bauer/The Wiley|date=2013-10-07|publisher=John Wiley & Sons Ltd|isbn=978-1-118-59770-5|editor-last=Bauer|editor-first=Patricia J.|___location=Chichester, UK|language=en|doi=10.1002/9781118597705|editor-last2=Fivush|editor-first2=Robyn}}</ref> A studied yet still speculative thought about episodic memory in children is the lack of foresight episodic.<ref>{{Cite book|url=http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/9781118597705|title=The Wiley Handbook on the Development of Children's Memory: Bauer/The Wiley|date=2013-10-07|publishername="John Wiley & Sons Ltd|isbn=978-1-118-59770-5|editor-last=Bauer|editor-first=Patricia J.|___location=Chichester, UK|language=en|doi=10.1002"/9781118597705|editor-last2=Fivush|editor-first2=Robyn}}</ref> This suggests that children are more susceptible and successful in remembering certain events (eg., what are you going to have for lunch, what will you play with in the park, etc.), not because they traveled in both past and future, but that parents are the ones’ who generally organize the day, meaning they are the ones who have control over their futures. <ref>{{Cite book|url=http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/9781118597705|title=The Wiley Handbook on the Development of Children's Memory: Bauer/The Wiley|date=2013-10-07|publishername="John Wiley & Sons Ltd|isbn=978-1-118-59770-5|editor-last=Bauer|editor-first=Patricia J.|___location=Chichester, UK|language=en|doi=10.1002"/9781118597705|editor-last2=Fivush|editor-first2=Robyn}}</ref>
 
==Autobiographical Memory==
Line 61:
 
===Childhood Amnesia===
Childhood amnesia is a phenomenon that ranges from the age of 3-83–8 years of age.<ref name="ReferenceA">{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/888026377|title=Handbook of child psychology and developmental science|others=Lerner, Richard M.,|isbn=978-1-118-95296-2|edition=Seventh edition|___location=Hoboken, New Jersey|oclc=888026377}}</ref> This phenomenon occurs when a child has forgotten memories and cannot recall them.<ref>{{Cite book|urlname=https:"ReferenceA"//www.worldcat.org/oclc/888026377|title=Handbook of child psychology and developmental science|others=Lerner, Richard M.,|isbn=978-1-118-95296-2|edition=Seventh edition|___location=Hoboken, New Jersey|oclc=888026377}}</ref> When a child goes through this, what they experience is non-retrievable information or memories from their storage.<ref>{{Cite book|urlname=https:"ReferenceA"//www.worldcat.org/oclc/888026377|title=Handbook of child psychology and developmental science|others=Lerner, Richard M.,|isbn=978-1-118-95296-2|edition=Seventh edition|___location=Hoboken, New Jersey|oclc=888026377}}</ref> For instance, when a certain event is forgotten, it can be accessible in our storage and the time limit depending of other factors, would be over a time of month or perhaps a year.<ref>{{Cite book|urlname=https:"ReferenceA"//www.worldcat.org/oclc/888026377|title=Handbook of child psychology and developmental science|others=Lerner, Richard M.,|isbn=978-1-118-95296-2|edition=Seventh edition|___location=Hoboken, New Jersey|oclc=888026377}}</ref> In amnesia it is not easily accessible. <ref>{{Citation|last=Pennington|first=Bruce F.|title=Atypical Cognitive Development|date=2015-03-23|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy223|work=Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science|pages=1–48|publisher=John Wiley & Sons, Inc.|isbn=978-1-118-96341-8|access-date=2020-03-30}}</ref>
 
[[Infantile amnesia]] is the tendency to have few autobiographical memories from below the age of 2-4. This can be attributed to lack of [[memory rehearsal]] as young children do not engage in rehearsal of remembered information. There are two theoretical explanations for why this may occur; although they take different approaches, they are not [[mutually exclusive]] of each other.<ref name="PsychBook">{{cite book|last=Robinson-Riegler; Robinson-Riegler|first=Bridget; Gregory|title=Cognitive Psychology: Applying the Science of the Mind|year=2012|publisher=Pearson Education Inc. as Allyn & Bacon|___location=75 Arlington Street, Suite 300, Boston, MA|isbn=978-0-205-17674-8|pages=272–276; 295–296; 339–346|edition=Third}}</ref> The development of a cognitive self is also thought by some to have an effect on encoding and storing early memories.<ref>[https://books.google.com/books?id=MB_PBwAAQBAJ&pg=PA218&lpg=PA218&dq=The+development+of+a+cognitive+self+is+also+thought+by+some+to+have+an+effect+on+encoding+and+storing+early+memories&source=bl&ots=sSw1xhUFaY&sig=tGznp7eKE58c--jLd1Mo0FF6lG0&hl=en&sa=X&ved=0ahUKEwit0Ij03u7LAhWCOSYKHfMUCdUQ6AEIKDAC#v=onepage&q=The%20development%20of%20a%20cognitive%20self%20is%20also%20thought%20by%20some%20to%20have%20an%20effect%20on%20encoding%20and%20storing%20early%20memories&f=false Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, Cognitive Processes]{{full citation needed|date=August 2016}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Loftus |first1=Elizabeth F. |title=Desperately seeking memories of the first few years of childhood: The reality of early memories |journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: General |volume=122 |issue=2 |year=1993 |pages=274–7 |doi=10.1037/0096-3445.122.2.274 |pmid=8315402}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Usher |first1=JoNell A. |last2=Neisser |first2=Ulric |title=Childhood amnesia and the beginnings of memory for four early life events |journal=Journal of Experimental Psychology: General |volume=122 |issue=2 |year=1993 |pages=155–65 |doi=10.1037/0096-3445.122.2.155 |pmid=8315398}}</ref>
 
====Cognitive Self====
Line 87:
At this age, children spontaneously use rehearsal to enhance short-term memory performance and retrieval strategies begin to be used spontaneously without the guidance of others.<ref name="paris" />
 
Elementary-age children have a significant improvement in their ability to retain information. <ref name="Bergin, Christi Ann Crosby">{{Cite book|last=Bergin, Christi Ann Crosby,|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/953710158|title=Child and adolescent development in your classroom : topical approach|others=Bergin, David Allen,|isbn=978-1-305-96424-2|edition=Third edition|___location=Boston, MA, US|oclc=953710158}}</ref> Children start to understand that in order to not forget what they have learned. It is crucial to create a connection that will not aid them to remember next time. <ref>{{Cite book|lastname="Bergin, Christi Ann Crosby,|url=https:"//www.worldcat.org/oclc/953710158|title=Child and adolescent development in your classroom : topical approach|others=Bergin, David Allen,|isbn=978-1-305-96424-2|edition=Third edition|___location=Boston, MA, US|oclc=953710158}}</ref> Once this skill has been learned or there has been significant progression, children in this age steadily become better at remembering to do things in the future (e.g., throwing out the garbage, closing the bathroom door or doing homework).<ref>{{Cite book|lastname="Bergin, Christi Ann Crosby,|url=https:"//www.worldcat.org/oclc/953710158|title=Child and adolescent development in your classroom : topical approach|others=Bergin, David Allen,|isbn=978-1-305-96424-2|edition=Third edition|___location=Boston, MA, US|oclc=953710158}}</ref> Children in this stage of their lives often have an attention shifting episodes in where it allows the portion of the memory that was expiring to activate once more, not allowing them to forget.<ref>{{Cite book|lastname="Bergin, Christi Ann Crosby,|url=https:"//www.worldcat.org/oclc/953710158|title=Child and adolescent development in your classroom : topical approach|others=Bergin, David Allen,|isbn=978-1-305-96424-2|edition=Third edition|___location=Boston, MA, US|oclc=953710158}}</ref> Once ascending the 3rd grade children are generally categorizing or organization to aid memory. <ref>{{Cite book|lastname="Bergin, Christi Ann Crosby,|url=https:"//www.worldcat.org/oclc/953710158|title=Child and adolescent development in your classroom : topical approach|others=Bergin, David Allen,|isbn=978-1-305-96424-2|edition=Third edition|___location=Boston, MA, US|oclc=953710158}}</ref>
 
====Late Elementary School====
In late elementary school, children engage in self-directed use of organization and demonstrate the ability to impose a [[semantic]] structure on the to-be-remembered items to guide memory performance. For example, if a child is packing their bag for school they can go through each part of their day and think of each item that they need to pack.<ref name="paris" /> Children at this age understand the advantages of using memory strategies and make use of strategies like categorization over looking or naming if they are instructed to think about learning strategies prior to learning.<ref name="justice" />
 
A strong metacognitive strategy for a student would be practicing reflective and critical thinking. <ref name="ReferenceB">{{Cite book|url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/1022637397|title=The SAGE encyclopedia of intellectual and developmental disorders|others=Braaten, Ellen,|isbn=978-1-4833-9227-1|___location=Thousand Oaks, California|oclc=1022637397}}</ref> For instance, when a child is asked to memorize a song or a poem as Forchelli (2018) points out, the teacher will sing or read aloud.<ref>{{Cite book|urlname=https:"ReferenceB"//www.worldcat.org/oclc/1022637397|title=The SAGE encyclopedia of intellectual and developmental disorders|others=Braaten, Ellen,|isbn=978-1-4833-9227-1|___location=Thousand Oaks, California|oclc=1022637397}}</ref> What is forming in the student’s mind is understanding the meaning of the words and what a connection made.<ref>{{Cite book|urlname=https:"ReferenceB"//www.worldcat.org/oclc/1022637397|title=The SAGE encyclopedia of intellectual and developmental disorders|others=Braaten, Ellen,|isbn=978-1-4833-9227-1|___location=Thousand Oaks, California|oclc=1022637397}}</ref> Once the student is practicing, he or she will be able to approach the song or poem with more understanding, reflective and problem solving. <ref>{{Cite book|urlname=https:"ReferenceB"//www.worldcat.org/oclc/1022637397|title=The SAGE encyclopedia of intellectual and developmental disorders|others=Braaten, Ellen,|isbn=978-1-4833-9227-1|___location=Thousand Oaks, California|oclc=1022637397}}</ref>
 
====Early Adolescence====
In early adolescence, children begin to use elaborative rehearsal meaning that items are not simply kept in mind but rather are processed more deeply. They also prefer to use memory strategies such as [[categorization]] rather than simple rehearsal, looking or naming and use these strategies without needing to think about memory strategies prior to learning.<ref name="justice" />
 
Consequently, it is crucial to acknowledge that a child’s brain is constantly experiencing development from life adaptation. <ref name="Damon 2005 15–19">{{Cite journal|last=Damon|first=William|date=2005|title=Looking back, for a change: A story of directions in child and adolescent development|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cd.133|journal=New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development|volume=2005|issue=109|pages=15–19|doi=10.1002/cd.133|issn=1520-3247}}</ref> Children need to be an environment that fortifies and encourages a cognitive development at an early age. <ref>{{Cite journal|last=UNCTAD-ICTSD|date=2005-04-07|title=Resource Book on TRIPS and Development|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511511363|doi=10.1017/cbo9780511511363}}</ref> However, in proportion too literature a child’s mind is a remarkable mechanism that if a child has not been adequately given the optimal care and stimulation for brain development. <ref>{{Cite journal|lastname="Damon|first=William|date= 2005|title=Looking back, for a change: A story of directions in child and adolescent development|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cd.133|journal=New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development|volume=2005|issue=109|pages=15–19|doi=10.1002"/cd.133|issn=1520-3247}}</ref> A child can inverse the damage sustained in their early life and have an opportunity to develop. <ref>{{Cite journal|lastname="Damon|first=William|date= 2005|title=Looking back, for a change: A story of directions in child and adolescent development|url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cd.133|journal=New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development|volume=2005|issue=109|pages=15–19|doi=10.1002"/cd.133|issn=1520-3247}}</ref>
 
==References==