Content deleted Content added
m Open access bot: doi added to citation with #oabot. |
Badams8521 (talk | contribs) m Minor grammatical edits |
||
Line 1:
{{short description|type of implicit long-term memory which aids the performance of particular types of tasks}}
{{Use dmy dates|date=October 2014}}
'''Procedural memory''' is a type of [[implicit memory]] (unconscious memory) and [[long-term memory]]; which aids the performance of particular types of tasks without conscious awareness of these previous experiences.
Procedural memory guides the processes we perform, and most frequently resides below the level of conscious awareness. When needed, procedural memories are automatically retrieved and utilized for the execution of the integrated procedures involved in both cognitive and motor skills, from tying shoes to flying an airplane and from drawing to reading. Procedural memories are accessed and used without the need for conscious control or attention.
Procedural memory is created through '''procedural learning,''' or
==History==
The difference between procedural and [[declarative memory]] systems
Psychologists and [[philosophers]] began writing about memory over two centuries ago. "Mechanical memory" was first noted in 1804 by [[Maine de Biran]]. [[William James]], within his famous book: ''[[The Principles of Psychology]]'' (1890), suggested that there was a difference between memory and habit. [[Cognitive psychology]] disregarded the influence of learning on memory systems in its early years, and this greatly limited the research conducted in procedural learning up until the 20th century.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Bullemer | first1 = P. | last2 = Nissen | first2 = MJ. | last3 = Willingham | first3 = D.B. | year = 1989 | title = On the Development of Procedural Knowledge | url = | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition | volume = 15 | issue = 6| pages = 1047–1060 | doi=10.1037/0278-7393.15.6.1047}}</ref> The turn of the century brought a clearer understanding of the functions and structures involved in procedural memory acquisition, storage, and retrieval processes.
McDougall{{who|date=November 2018}} (1923) first made the distinction between [[explicit memory|explicit]] and implicit memory. In the 1970s procedural and declarative knowledge was distinguished in literature on [[artificial intelligence]]. Studies in the 1970s divided and moved towards two areas of work: one focusing on animal studies and the other to amnesic patients. The first convincing experimental evidence for a dissociation between [[declarative memory]] ("knowing what") and non-declarative or procedural ("knowing how") memory was from Milner (1962), by demonstrating that a severely amnesic patient, [[Henry Molaison]], formerly known as patient H.M., could learn a hand–eye coordination skill (mirror drawing) in the absence of any memory of having practiced the task before. Although this finding indicated that memory was not made up of a single system positioned in one place in the brain, at the time, others agreed that motor skills are likely a special case that represented a less cognitive form of memory. However, by refining and improving experimental measures, there has been extensive research using amnesic patients with varying locations and degrees of structural damage. Increased work with amnesic patients led to the finding that they were able to retain and learn tasks other than motor skills. However, these findings had shortcomings in how they were perceived as amnesic patients sometimes fell short on normal levels of performance and therefore [[amnesia]] was viewed as strictly a retrieval deficit. Further studies with amnesic patients found a larger ___domain of normally functioning memory for skill abilities. For example, using a mirror reading task, amnesic patients showed performance at a normal rate, even though they are unable to remember some of the words that they were reading. In the 1980s much was discovered about the anatomy physiology of the mechanisms involved in procedural memory. The [[cerebellum]], [[hippocampus]], [[neostriatum]], and [[basal ganglia]] were identified as being involved in memory acquisition tasks.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Squire | first1 = L.R. | year = 2004 | title = Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective | url = | journal = Neurobiology of Learning and Memory | volume = 82 | issue = 3| pages = 171–177 | doi=10.1016/j.nlm.2004.06.005| pmid = 15464402 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.319.8326 }}</ref>
|